Review Special Issues

A dual-brain therapeutic approach using noninvasive brain stimulation based on two-person neuroscience: A perspective review


  • Our actions and decisions in everyday life are heavily influenced by social interactions, which are dynamic feedback loops involving actions, reactions, and internal cognitive processes between individual agents. Social interactions induce interpersonal synchrony, which occurs at different biobehavioral levels and comprises behavioral, physiological, and neurological activities. Hyperscanning—a neuroimaging technique that simultaneously measures the activity of multiple brain regions—has provided a powerful second-person neuroscience tool for investigating the phase alignment of neural processes during interactive social behavior. Neural synchronization, revealed by hyperscanning, is a phenomenon called inter-brain synchrony- a process that purportedly facilitates social interactions by prompting appropriate anticipation of and responses to each other's social behaviors during ongoing shared interactions. In this review, I explored the therapeutic dual-brain approach using noninvasive brain stimulation to target inter-brain synchrony based on second-person neuroscience to modulate social interaction. Artificially inducing synchrony between the brains is a potential adjunct technique to physiotherapy, psychotherapy, and pain treatment- which are strongly influenced by the social interaction between the therapist and patient. Dual-brain approaches to personalize stimulation parameters must consider temporal, spatial, and oscillatory factors. Multiple data fusion analysis, the assessment of inter-brain plasticity, a closed-loop system, and a brain-to-brain interface can support personalized stimulation.

    Citation: Naoyuki Takeuchi. A dual-brain therapeutic approach using noninvasive brain stimulation based on two-person neuroscience: A perspective review[J]. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2024, 21(4): 5118-5137. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2024226

    Related Papers:

    [1] Baba Issa Camara, Houda Mokrani, Evans K. Afenya . Mathematical modeling of glioma therapy using oncolytic viruses. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2013, 10(3): 565-578. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2013.10.565
    [2] Yunyuan Gao, Zhen Cao, Jia Liu, Jianhai Zhang . A novel dynamic brain network in arousal for brain states and emotion analysis. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2021, 18(6): 7440-7463. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2021368
    [3] Wei Yin, Tao Yang, GuangYu Wan, Xiong Zhou . Identification of image genetic biomarkers of Alzheimer's disease by orthogonal structured sparse canonical correlation analysis based on a diagnostic information fusion. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2023, 20(9): 16648-16662. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2023741
    [4] Sridevi Sriram, Hayder Natiq, Karthikeyan Rajagopal, Ondrej Krejcar, Hamidreza Namazi . Dynamics of a two-layer neuronal network with asymmetry in coupling. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2023, 20(2): 2908-2919. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2023137
    [5] Gayathri Vivekanandhan, Hamid Reza Abdolmohammadi, Hayder Natiq, Karthikeyan Rajagopal, Sajad Jafari, Hamidreza Namazi . Dynamic analysis of the discrete fractional-order Rulkov neuron map. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2023, 20(3): 4760-4781. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2023220
    [6] Daniel Cervantes, Miguel angel Moreles, Joaquin Peña, Alonso Ramirez-Manzanares . A computational method for the covariance matrix associated with extracellular diffusivity on disordered models of cylindrical brain axons. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2021, 18(5): 4961-4970. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2021252
    [7] Benjamin Steinberg, Yuqing Wang, Huaxiong Huang, Robert M. Miura . Spatial Buffering Mechanism: Mathematical Model and Computer Simulations. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2005, 2(4): 675-702. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2005.2.675
    [8] Haiqiang Liu, Feifei Shen, Hewei Zhang, Weikai Zhang . Expression and role of cystatin C in hyperthermia-induced brain injury in rats. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2023, 20(2): 2716-2731. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2023127
    [9] Changwei Gong, Bing Xue, Changhong Jing, Chun-Hui He, Guo-Cheng Wu, Baiying Lei, Shuqiang Wang . Time-sequential graph adversarial learning for brain modularity community detection. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2022, 19(12): 13276-13293. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2022621
    [10] Yuqing Zhang, Yutong Han, Jianxin Zhang . MAU-Net: Mixed attention U-Net for MRI brain tumor segmentation. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2023, 20(12): 20510-20527. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2023907
  • Our actions and decisions in everyday life are heavily influenced by social interactions, which are dynamic feedback loops involving actions, reactions, and internal cognitive processes between individual agents. Social interactions induce interpersonal synchrony, which occurs at different biobehavioral levels and comprises behavioral, physiological, and neurological activities. Hyperscanning—a neuroimaging technique that simultaneously measures the activity of multiple brain regions—has provided a powerful second-person neuroscience tool for investigating the phase alignment of neural processes during interactive social behavior. Neural synchronization, revealed by hyperscanning, is a phenomenon called inter-brain synchrony- a process that purportedly facilitates social interactions by prompting appropriate anticipation of and responses to each other's social behaviors during ongoing shared interactions. In this review, I explored the therapeutic dual-brain approach using noninvasive brain stimulation to target inter-brain synchrony based on second-person neuroscience to modulate social interaction. Artificially inducing synchrony between the brains is a potential adjunct technique to physiotherapy, psychotherapy, and pain treatment- which are strongly influenced by the social interaction between the therapist and patient. Dual-brain approaches to personalize stimulation parameters must consider temporal, spatial, and oscillatory factors. Multiple data fusion analysis, the assessment of inter-brain plasticity, a closed-loop system, and a brain-to-brain interface can support personalized stimulation.



    Our everyday actions and decisions are heavily influenced by communicative behaviors with others such as cooperating, competing, imitating, helping, playing, providing information, asking questions, negotiating, bargaining, and bluffing [1]. These communications are called social interactions and are dynamic feedback loops that link actions, reactions, and internal cognitive processes between individual agents [2,3]. Social interactions are associated with interpersonal synchrony that occurs at multiple biobehavioral levels of behavioral, physiological, and hormonal activity [4,5,6,7]. Moreover, temporally simultaneous patterns of cognitive alignment and communicative behaviors during social interactions can align neural activities across individuals [2,8].

    The focus of studies on the neural mechanisms underlying social interactions has shifted from single individuals to interactions between individuals. This paradigm shift is known as "second-person neuroscience" or "interpersonal neuroscience" [9,10]. Hyperscanning- a neuroimaging technique that simultaneously measures the activity of multiple brains- is a powerful second-person neuroscience tool for investigating the phase alignment of neural processes during social interactions. Neural synchronization revealed by hyperscanning is a phenomenon called inter-brain synchrony (IBS), which purportedly supports social interaction by aligning the neural activity of dyads [7,11] and groups [12,13]. IBS may be associated with the transfer of information between brains, as the phase alignment of neural activities facilitates the efficient transfer of information between multiple regions within a single brain [14]. This may represent an adaptive capacity that allows people to access the internal arousal state of others; share and regulate emotions; enhance social affiliation, empathy, and prosocial engagement; and facilitate learning [7,8,15,16].

    Human hyperscanning studies have identified several brain regions involved in IBS, including the prefrontal cortex (PFC) [17,18], anterior cingulate cortex [19], superior temporal gyrus [20,21], temporoparietal junction (TPJ) [22], and insular cortex [23]. Brain regions associated with IBS are involved in theory of mind [24], predictive processing [25], mirroring [26], and social cognition [27], which have been shown to play important roles in social interactions [28,29]. These hyperscanning results support the hypothesis that IBS reflects complex cognitive processes, including mentalization, prediction, imitation, and simulation of behavioral and affective states during social interactions [7]. These brain regions are expected targets for the modulation of human social functions using noninvasive brain stimulation (NIBS), which can change cortical excitability using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation [30,31,32]. However, few direct associations have been identified with the rich field of NIBS, although the development of second-person neuroscience has provided opportunities to uncover the neural mechanisms of social interactions and to study how interpersonal brain activity shapes social behavior. Rather than stimulating a single brain, a dual-brain approach using NIBS across individuals can help control social interactions by manipulating communication between the brains.

    This review explored the possibility of a dual-brain therapeutic approach using NIBS to modulate social interactions based on the current knowledge of IBS. First, it provides an overview of the methods used to analyze IBS through hyperscanning, a neuroimaging technique that simultaneously measures the activity of multiple brains. Multimodal data fusion analysis of behavioral, physiological, and neural activity across individuals- helps elucidate the neurobiological mechanisms of IBS. This review discusses multi-brain stimulation (MBS) using NIBS to modulate inter-brain communication. The simultaneous manipulation of brain activity helps elucidate the causal role of IBS in social interactions. Next, I discuss the possibility of clinical application using the dual-brain approach for facilitating social interactions as an adjunctive technique to physiotherapy, psychotherapy, and pain treatment, the effectiveness of which is strongly influenced by interactive communication between the therapist and patient. Finally, I discuss the future directions of dual-brain approaches to personalize stimulation parameters by considering temporal, spatial, and oscillatory factors.

    Electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG), functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) are used to measure multibrain activity in hyperscanning research. EEG and MEG record cortical activity with a high temporal resolution, allowing the study of high-frequency dynamics and network patterns. fNIRS can be used to record the cortical hemodynamic activity without strict behavioral constraints. Compared to other neuroimaging techniques, fMRI offers the best opportunity for brain-wide access to neural signals; however, its technical limitations exclude tasks involving physical interactions [2,3]. Table 1 lists the commonly used analytical methods and provides a brief description of their characteristics. This review was not intended for detailing any of the IBS assessment methods. I recommend the excellent earlier reviews for further details [3,33,34,35,36].

    Table 1.  Several commonly used analytical methods for assessment of inter-brain synchrony.
    Analytical method Measuring Characteristics Example reference
    Phase Locking Value (PLV) Consistency of phase synchrony between neural signals Analysis value is the average of all phase differences for each time point in a trial [19,37,38,39]
    Inter-brain Phase Coherence (IPC) Phase synchrony between neural signals based on a traditional Fourier method Analysis value is calculated based on a whole trial [40,41]
    Partial Directed Coherence (PDC) Direction of information flow based on multivariate autoregressive modeling PDC provides causal information in frequency domain, with multi-channel data [42,43,44]
    Pearson (or Spearman) correlation coefficient Similarities between neural signals estimated via linear dependence Analysis value is high when the data are dependent and low when the relationship is highly non-linear [22,45,46]
    Circular correlation coefficient (CCorr) Circular covariance of differences between the observed and mean phases CCorr is robust to coincidental synchronization [47,48]
    Wavelet Transform Coherence (WTC) Local coherence between neural signals as a function of both frequency and time WTC can capture out-of-phase relationships between data and reveal frequency information [18,49,50,51,52]
    Inter-subject Correlation (ISC) Similarity between neural signals in one region between brains ISC is useful for naturalistic experimental paradigms because it does not require an explicit model of task or stimulus [53,54]
    Inter-subject Functional Connectivity (ISFC) Similarity between neural signals in one region of one brain and all other regions in another brain ISFC, obtained by calculating ISCs between each voxel and all other voxels, can quantify neural similarity across brains [55]
    Granger Causality Analysis (GC) Direction of information flow from one brain to another GC can represent the linear relationship between data and is the most common method of inferring causality [49,50]

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Several analytical methods are used to estimate the covariance or directional neural coupling of the time series produced by two interacting individuals on EEG and MEG. Common analyses of IBS in electrophysiological studies use intra-brain estimators such as the Phase Locking Value (PLV) [56], Inter-brain Phase Coherence (IPC) [40], and Partial Directed Coherence (PDC) [44]. While PLV and IPC measure the phase synchrony between neural signals, PDC is useful for determining the direction of synchrony between neural signals in a dyad [33,34]. The other IBS analyses used include Pearson's correlation coefficient, Spearman's correlation coefficient, circular correlation coefficient, and wavelet transform coherence (WTC) [34]. Time series correlation analyses that measure symmetric effects provide a simple measure of shared dynamics between brains but may miss out-of-phase synchronous relationships [2]. This is because many social interactions are asymmetrical with two participants playing different roles [57]. The WTC was developed for the analysis of geophysical time series [58] and has mainly been used for the analysis of fNIRS hyperscanning studies [33]. Analyses using fMRI hyperscanning include coherence analysis [21,59], inter-subject correlation [54], and inter-subject functional connectivity [55]. Other analyses such as Granger causality and dynamic causal modeling, have been used to reveal temporal relationships and inter-brain information flow [60,61,62]. Although there are various methods of analysis, as described above, and each estimation, analysis, and hyperscanning technique has its advantages and limitations, there is no uniform method for assessing IBS [33,34]. In addition, there is a lack of studies comparing the quantitative results of IBS in the field of hyperscanning research [36]. The lack of established standard methods makes it difficult to resolve the issues arising from ambiguity in the definition and theory of IBS. Therefore, common analysis guidelines should be established to facilitate the replication of results and their interpretation [63,64].

    The behavioral coding approach is helpful for quantifying the extent to which specific communicative behaviors contribute to IBS, thereby interpreting neural-behavioral relationships [57,65,66,67]. However, the subjective nature of behavioral coding raises reliability concerns, even when a detailed coding scheme is provided and multiple raters participate. Therefore, the use of automated algorithms, such as Motion Energy Analysis and OpenPose, to extract behavioral information from video recordings should be considered. Motion Energy Analysis is a computer vision technique based on assessing differences in sequences of frames in video recordings, which can provide a continuous measure of motion [68]. The OpenPose system is a real-time multiperson 2D pose estimation system that uses Part Affinity Fields and is capable of tracking human interactions from the body, foot, hand, and face keypoints in images or videos [69]. Moreover, for behavioral activities, IBS is comodulated by physiological activities such as heart rate, pupil size, and saccade rate [6]. Therefore, IBS assessment techniques should be developed to capture rich information about physiological changes in addition to participants' behaviors such as facial expressions and hand and body movements [57,65,70]. Integrating data that capture behavioral, physiological, and neural interactions between individuals can reveal the underlying mechanisms of social interactions that cannot be inferred from IBS alone [7,57]. In addition, the machine learning-based decoding approach, which builds mathematical models based on sample data to automatically classify activities, can provide valuable information on whether IBS serves as a neural classification feature that optimizes the decoding of interpersonal multimodal activities during social interactions [8].

    IBS, revealed by hyperscanning, may be a neural mechanism that facilitates social interactions by prompting appropriate anticipation and response to each other's social behaviors during ongoing shared interactions [71,72]. Therefore, artificially modulating IBS using neurofeedback and MBS has the potential to improve social interaction. Multi-brain neurofeedback using brain-computer interface technology involves providing real-time feedback of brain activity to interacting individuals, potentially enabling them to regulate IBS [73,74,75]. Some brain dynamics observed during the multi-brain neurofeedback task correlated with communicative behaviors, suggesting a link between cognitive and emotional states [73]. However, multi-brain neurofeedback approaches have the disadvantage that feedback learning takes time; and detailed phase shifts are not easily implemented. However, MBS using NIBS can easily reproduce different stimulation phase shifts between the brains and respond to cases in which the oscillatory activity between the brains may not be fully synchronized. In humans, rhythmic NIBS such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) can be used to simultaneously stimulate multiple brains by entraining brain oscillations. TMS creates a local magnetic field to stimulate the cortex through the scalp using wire coils placed on the scalp. When a pulsed magnetic field enters the brain, it creates an electric current that flows through neurons, causing them to depolarize [76]. In contrast, tACS uses a weak oscillating current stimulation applied to the brain via scalp electrodes to drive neuronal activity into these frequency patterns [77,78]. These NIBS entrainment designs generally match the stimulation frequencies to those of the underlying brain activity because computational modeling suggests that brain activity entrapment is characterized by an increased likelihood of "resonance" of oscillatory brain activity when the stimulation frequency matches that of the underlying brain oscillations [79,80]. However, no studies have used TMS to manipulate communication between the brains, and tACS has mainly been used for MBS in human studies [81]. tACS can induce functional connectivity between distant regions of the brain by modulating the phase synchrony of oscillations [77,82]. As an application of intra- to inter-brain communication, tACS applied simultaneously to two brains, known as hyper-tACS, aims to manipulate the oscillation frequencies between brains and artificially induce IBS.

    IBS has mainly been reported in slower frequency bands, such as theta (4–7 Hz) [19,41,83,84] and alpha-mu (8–13 Hz) [37,47,85,86,87], but it has also been reported in beta (14–30 Hz) [19,88,89] and gamma (30–80 Hz) bands [20,22,38,90]. Matching the stimulation frequency of the NIBS to the neural oscillations associated with the brain regions and behavioral contexts may be effective for entrainment; however, in the case of artificial IBS modulation, it is necessary to consider not only within the brain but also the interactions between brains. Interactions between brains can be thought of as long range, with low frequencies suitable for integrating information across distant areas. There are fewer constraints on timing accuracy at lower frequencies because the excitability rise and fall periods are longer [91,92]. Computational simulations also suggest that low frequencies such as theta oscillations are associated with the sharing of belief states when communicating [93]. However, the specific neural oscillations associated with IBS are currently not well understood, so for future MBS studies using tACS, I will outline the characteristics of other oscillation bands associated with IBS. The alpha-mu-band is the most robust oscillation in IBS during social interactions [94]. However, alpha-band oscillations are well-established neural activities related to attention [95,96]. Attention to relevant cues may enhance common neural processing, leading to meaningless synchronized neural oscillations unrelated to social interaction. Therefore, alpha IBS during social interactions should be interpreted with caution [63,97]. As beta-band oscillations are associated with motor processes [98,99], a relationship between beta oscillations and movement synchrony during social interaction might be expected. However, the results for the beta band IBS involved in movement synchrony have been inconsistent [19,40,89,100]. Gamma oscillations have been associated with several neurocognitive processes like: Information processing, conscious perception, and memory [101]. Given these relationships, the gamma band appears to be associated with cognitive processes, such as social interaction. However, this is too fast for the multiple brains to interact with. Even the slowest gamma frequencies oscillate within approximately 30 ms. Therefore, brains interacting at gamma frequencies must establish and maintain IBS with a half-cycle accuracy of at least 15 ms [63]. This is likely not possible given that the duration of the excitatory postsynaptic potential is approximately 10 ms [102]. Therefore, IBS at gamma frequencies has often reported, but its role in social interactions is questionable [63].

    Few studies have reported on hyper-tACS [103,104,105]. Novembre et al. reported that hyper-tACS increases interpersonal synchrony. In-phase 20-Hz tACS applied to the motor cortex in dyads performing a finger-tapping task improved interpersonal movement synchrony compared to anti-phase or sham stimulation. The phase coupling of brain oscillations across the motor cortices of two individuals may support the interpersonal alignment of sensorimotor processes, resulting in the facilitation of interpersonal synchrony [103]. Pan et al. reported that hyper-tACS applied to the inferior frontal cortex improved instruction-based learning. In-phase 6-Hz hyper-tACS applied to the instructor and learner improved the learning performance of naturalistic song-learning tasks [105]. In addition, body movement synchrony of the dyads was increased by hyper-tACS. In contrast, another study reported that in-phase 6-Hz hyper-tACS applied to the right frontocentral and centroparietal sites during a dyadic drumming synchrony task worsened interpersonal action coordination [104]. The hyper-tACS frequency, location, and phase may have been inappropriate for facilitating this dyadic task since it deteriorated the interpersonal coordination. Furthermore, it may not be necessary for the two brains to synchronize at the same frequency. Brain oscillations can be coupled with activity in other brains at lower frequencies, known as cross-frequency coupling (CFC), where one stimulation band modulates another band between different regions of the brain [106].

    Thus, how neural oscillations are coupled between brains remains poorly understood, and future studies on dual-brain approaches using NIBS to modulate social interactions will need to consider the possibility of temporal, spatial, and frequency asymmetries in brain activity between participants. Additionally, electrical stimulation applied to the scalp, such as tACS, is often associated with peripheral sensations [107]. Therefore, brain regions unrelated to task function should be examined as controls to exclude the possibility that changes in social interactions by hyper-tACS are due to sensory stimulation effects. Furthermore, future studies should investigate whether the experimental modulation of IBS could affect the degree of social interaction to deny that IBS is not purely epiphenomenally induced by other forms of neural synchrony, such as behavioral entrainment, cognitive alignment, shared understanding, and affective contagion. Experimental hyper-tACS, which simultaneously manipulates neural activity in interacting partners, could help clarify the causal role of IBS in social interactions by modulating IBS and measuring its influence on social interactions [108]. However, the potential causal mechanisms of IBS, including behavioral entrainment, shared understanding, and affective contagion, are not mutually exclusive and may collaborate to facilitate neural coupling [8].

    The effectiveness of psychotherapy, physiotherapy, and pain treatment is influenced by the therapist–patient relationship [109,110,111,112]. Therefore, the dual-brain approach using NIBS aimed at facilitating social interaction may be an adjunctive approach in clinical situations that are strongly influenced by interactive communication with others. Impaired social interactions are one of the most common symptoms of many psychiatric disorders. The study of the neural mechanisms underlying interactions between individuals in the field of psychiatry is referred to as second-person neuropsychiatry [113,114]. Disturbances in inter-brain dynamics may have a negative impact on social function, because brain-brain coupling may trigger neural mechanisms that control social alignment, such as cooperation and affective contagion. In line with this, it has been investigated how the dynamics between the brains may be altered in mental and neurodevelopmental disorders associated with social deficits. Previous studies reported abnormal IBS during interactions between healthy individuals and patients with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by deficits in interpersonal interactions and social communication [51,115]. Wang et al. reported that children with more severe ASD symptoms showed lower IBS levels in the PFC with their parents during cooperative tasks [51]. In addition to ASD, IBS is reduced in the right TPJ during the cooperation task in healthy individuals and patients with borderline personality disorders compared to those observed in healthy dyads [116]. Furthermore, low IBS has not been observed between healthy individuals and remitted patients with borderline personality disorders. Using fNIRS hyperscanning, Zhang et al. found that IBS in the right TPJ between counselors and clients increased during psychological counseling, which correlated with the strength of the therapeutic alliance [117]. A deeper understanding of the dynamics between the brains of different patients may reveal the common neural mechanisms of different psychiatric disorders as well as diagnostic tools for patient heterogeneity, potentially leading to prosocial therapeutic approaches for psychotherapy.

    Social interactions between therapists and patients are important in physiotherapy, in which therapists instruct patients to improve their motor impairments. Instruction is not just a one-way transmission of information from instructors to learners; rather, it is a complex social interaction that requires mentalizing and metacognitive functions to understand others' intentions and monitor information about others in addition to oneself [118]. Much evidence suggests that the IBS in the PFC increases during instruction-based learning [17,18,119]. A meta-analysis of 16 hyperscanning studies also concluded that IBS levels during the teaching-learning interactions correlates with better learning outcomes [120]. The PFC plays a central role in metacognitive processes [121,122]. Furthermore, PFC is associated with shared intentionality, requiring individuals to align their thoughts with others to promote coordinated behavior [52]. Patients with brain damage, such as stroke and traumatic brain injury, who require physiotherapy, often have impaired metacognitive and mentalizing functions. These cognitive impairments lead to poor rehabilitation outcomes [123,124,125,126]. The interpersonal neural entrainment induced by oscillatory synchronization through phase alignment can facilitate the efficient transfer of information from one brain to another, resulting in better social interaction during instruction-based learning [66,127]. Consistent with this, in-phase theta hyper-tACS applied to the inferior frontal cortex in both instructors and learners enhanced interactive social learning [105]. Artificial induction of IBS can help physiotherapy by enhancing instruction-based learning between the therapist and patient.

    It is well known that interpersonal interactions relieve pain [128,129]. Pain empathy allows us to understand and recognize others' pain experiences by observing them and inducing the sharing of emotions related to pain relief [128,130]. A previous EEG hyperscanning study investigated the neural mechanisms underlying pain empathy when one participant in a dyad was presented with an electrical pain stimulus [39]. This study shows that the IBS of the sensorimotor alpha oscillation band between pain sufferers and observers was greater during the anticipation of high-intensity pain than during low-intensity pain. In addition, the mediation analysis showed that the sharing of a painful experience induced prosocial behavior within pairs of individuals via IBS. Another study using EEG hyperscanning found that when romantic partners held hands, the alpha oscillation band in the centro-parietal regions increased during pain stimulation [47]. IBS also correlated with the degree of pain relief and touch-related empathic accuracy of the observer. An fMRI hyperscanning study investigated the concordance of brain activity when clinicians treated evoked pain in patients with chronic pain [131]. Behavioral mirroring was reportedly associated with pain relief and therapeutic alliance between patient–clinician dyads and that dynamic concordance of brain activity increased in circuits associated with theory of mind and mirror processing. A good therapist–patient relationship improves patient outcomes and is an important component of psychological analgesia [112,132]. A dual-brain approach using NIBS, based on IBS knowledge, to manipulate social interactions may provide pain relief through prosocial effects.

    Thus, dual-brain approaches such as MBS have potentially broad clinical implications for mental health, rehabilitation, and pain treatment. However, before artificial manipulation of IBS based on second-person neuroscience can be applied clinically, a caveat regarding the therapeutic relationship needs to be considered. The dual-brain approach of the therapist–patient dyad to artificially modulate social interactions in clinical settings is likely to raise important ethical issues and requires systematic neuroethical guidelines [133,134]. Before using the dual-brain approach to improve the therapist–patient relationship, the issue of artificially manipulating the therapist–patient alliance must be discussed to ensure the therapist's neutrality toward the patient.

    The NIBS parameters used in the dual-brain approach should be personalized to stabilize and promote artificial induction of IBS. Longitudinal hyperscanning may allow more valid stimulus parameters across dyads by revealing how the temporal, spatial, and oscillatory nature of IBS changes with the task and/or participant as social interaction progresses. The use of brain-to-brain interfaces that allow decoded neural information to be exchanged between the two brains may be useful in the clinical dual-brain approach to avoid stimulating healthy therapists. A closed-loop system that monitors and predicts individual responses in real time can personalize the stimulus to maximize the effect. Studies on IBS have mainly focused on within-subject effects under different task conditions, and there is a lack of research on the differences in IBS between healthy individuals and therapeutic targets. Longitudinal hyperscanning and closed-loop systems may be beneficial for improving the precision of dual-brain approaches using NIBS to adequately account for inter-individual differences in IBS. In the final chapter, future directions of dual-brain approaches that use NIBS for personalized stimulation are discussed.

    Social interactions evolve over time and the coupling between brains may change over the course of one or more interactions. This idea is supported by the emerging concept of inter-brain plasticity, which is the ability of interacting brains to modify the coupling between brains in response to repeated social interactions [135]. Inter-brain plasticity may be influenced by the lesions and reorganization of the brain after injury, analogous to intra-brain plasticity. To address the influence of structural changes on inter-brain plasticity, source-based estimation of brain activity using techniques such as minimum norm estimation [136], low-resolution electromagnetic tomography [137], and blind source separation techniques such as independent component analysis [138] may be helpful in electrophysiological signal analysis. Moreover, longitudinal hyperscanning studies across multiple interventions may reveal how neural dynamics in the coupled brain change and are associated with functional improvements. This information and approach will help us improve our knowledge of IBS as an objective indicator of treatment efficacy and allow for more valid stimulus parameters across dyads while preserving subject-specific differences in analyses.

    Invasive neurostimulation is evident; however, the dual-brain approach using NIBS to stimulate the brain in healthy therapists is unrealistic in clinical settings. Rather than a treatment method, MBS is a useful technique for investigating the role of frequency- and location-specific IBS in social interactions, which can support and complement findings from hyperscanning and single-brain stimulation studies. Once the role of IBS in social interactions is clarified, a brain-to-brain interface may be useful for avoiding stimulating therapists. A brain-to-brain interface receives neural information from the brain and transmits it to the brain of another person via electrical stimulation [139]. Therefore, a brain-to-brain interface enables the manipulation of IBS by modulating the patient's brain activity using rhythmic forms of NIBS and adjusting it to the therapist's brain activity. The direct transmission of neural information could enable more bidirectional therapist–patient interactions, leading to improved functioning. Furthermore, it is desirable that the stimulus parameters for the patient are continuously adjusted according to the patient's brain state using a closed-loop control system. This system, which consists of a high-temporal-resolution recording of brain activity (EEG/MEG) and NIBS, could make it possible to predict individual neural responses to NIBS and adjust real-time personalized stimulation parameters such as location, frequency, and phase, to maximize the stimulation effect [140,141]. To establish bidirectional social interactions, it is important to coordinate both participants, rather than take a unilateral approach [142]. Providing the therapist with real-time feedback on IBS, in addition to stimulating the patient, may be useful for bidirectional social interactions. For practical applications, further refinement and testing are required to remove artifacts produced by tACS without removing large amounts of valuable electrophysiological signaling [140,143]. The figure shows a schema of the future direction of personalized stimulation methods for IBS modulation to facilitate clinical effects using multiple data fusion analysis, assessment of inter-brain plasticity, closed-loop stimulation, and brain-to-brain interface.

    Figure 1.  Schematic of the future direction for personalized stimulation.

    Multimodal data fusion analysis can further elucidate inter-brain synchrony in terms of neural–physiological-behavioral relationships. Assessing inter-brain plasticity can provide more valid stimulus parameters for the dual-brain approach. Brain-to-brain interfaces can support a therapeutic dual-brain approach to avoid stimulating a healthy therapist, and a closed-loop system can adjust stimulation parameters according to individual responses.

    An understanding of dynamic inter-brain interactions and their mechanisms provides deep insight into social interaction in clinical settings. Recent meta-analyses have identified brain regions associated with IBS during social interactions [7,11]; however, few direct associations with the rich field of NIBS have been identified. Dual-brain approaches using NIBS have broad clinical implications for mental health, rehabilitation, and pain treatment. New technologies, such as closed-loop stimulation and brain-to-brain interfaces, may help personalize stimulation to facilitate clinical effects. However, the neutrality of the therapist towards the patient should be maintained before the artificial manipulation of IBS based on second-person neuroscience can be applied clinically.

    The author declares that he has not used Artificial Intelligence (AI) tools in the creation of this article.

    This work was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (No. 20K11278) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science.

    The author declares that there are no conflicts of interest.



    [1] R. Hari, M. V. Kujala, Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging, Physiol. Rev., 89 (2009), 453–479. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00041.2007 doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2007
    [2] L. Kingsbury, W. Hong, A multi-brain framework for social interaction, Trends Neurosci., 43 (2020), 651–666. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2020.06.008 doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.06.008
    [3] L. Tsoi, S. M. Burns, E. B. Falk, D. I. Tamir, The promises and pitfalls of functional magnetic resonance imaging hyperscanning for social interaction research, Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass, 16 (2022), e12707. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12707 doi: 10.1111/spc3.12707
    [4] I. Gordon, S. Wallot, Y. Berson, Group-level physiological synchrony and individual-level anxiety predict positive affective behaviors during a group decision-making task, Psychophysiology, 58 (2021), e13857. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13857 doi: 10.1111/psyp.13857
    [5] V. Reindl, S. Wass, V. Leong, W. Scharke, S. Wistuba, C. L. Wirth, et al., Multimodal hyperscanning reveals that synchrony of body and mind are distinct in mother-child dyads, Neuroimage, 251 (2022), 118982. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118982 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118982
    [6] J. Madsen, L. C. Parra, Cognitive processing of a common stimulus synchronizes brains, hearts, and eyes, PNAS Nexus, 1 (2022), pgac020. https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgac020 doi: 10.1093/pnasnexus/pgac020
    [7] L. D. Lotter, S. H. Kohl, C. Gerloff, L. Bell, A. Niephaus, J. A. Kruppa, et al., Revealing the neurobiology underlying interpersonal neural synchronization with multimodal data fusion, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 146 (2023), 105042. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105042 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105042
    [8] Y. Pan, G. Novembre, A. Olsson, The interpersonal neuroscience of social learning, Perspect. Psychol. Sci., 17 (2022), 680–695. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211008429 doi: 10.1177/17456916211008429
    [9] E. Redcay, L. Schilbach, Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction, Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 20 (2019), 495–505. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0179-4 doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0179-4
    [10] L. Schilbach, B. Timmermans, V. Reddy, A. Costall, G. Bente, T. Schlicht, et al., Toward a second-person neuroscience, Behav. Brain Sci., 36 (2013), 393–414. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x12000660 doi: 10.1017/s0140525x12000660
    [11] A. Czeszumski, S. H. Liang, S. Dikker, P. König, C. P. Lee, S. L. Koole, et al., Cooperative behavior evokes interbrain synchrony in the prefrontal and temporoparietal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis of fNIRS hyperscanning studies, eNeuro, 9 (2022), ENEURO.0268-21.2022. https://doi.org/10.1523/eneuro.0268-21.2022 doi: 10.1523/eneuro.0268-21.2022
    [12] S. Dikker, L. Wan, I. Davidesco, L. Kaggen, M. Oostrik, J. McClintock, et al., Brain-to-brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom, Curr. Biol., 27 (2017), 1375–1380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002
    [13] D. A. Reinero, S. Dikker, J. J. Van Bavel, Inter-brain synchrony in teams predicts collective performance, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 16 (2021), 43–57. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa135 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa135
    [14] P. Fries, Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence, Neuron, 88 (2015), 220–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034
    [15] M. Zee, H. M. Koomen, I. Van der Veen, Student-teacher relationship quality and academic adjustment in upper elementary school: the role of student personality, J. School Psychol., 51 (2013), 517–533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2013.05.003 doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2013.05.003
    [16] R. Mogan, R. Fischer, J. A. Bulbulia, To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony's effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect, J. Exp. Social Psychol., 72 (2017), 13–20. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.03.009
    [17] J. Liu, R. Zhang, B. Geng, T. Zhang, D. Yuan, S. Otani, et al., Interplay between prior knowledge and communication mode on teaching effectiveness: Interpersonal neural synchronization as a neural marker, Neuroimage, 193 (2019), 93–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.004 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.004
    [18] Y. Pan, S. Dikker, P. Goldstein, Y. Zhu, C. Yang, Y. Hu, Instructor-learner brain coupling discriminates between instructional approaches and predicts learning, Neuroimage, 211 (2020), 116657. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116657 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116657
    [19] K. Yun, K. Watanabe, S. Shimojo, Interpersonal body and neural synchronization as a marker of implicit social interaction, Sci. Rep., 2 (2012), 959. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep00959 doi: 10.1038/srep00959
    [20] J. Levy, A. Goldstein, R. Feldman, Perception of social synchrony induces mother-child gamma coupling in the social brain, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 12 (2017), 1036–1046. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx032 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx032
    [21] A. Stolk, M. L. Noordzij, L. Verhagen, I. Volman, J. M. Schoffelen, R. Oostenveld, et al., Cerebral coherence between communicators marks the emergence of meaning, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 111 (2014), 18183–18188. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1414886111 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414886111
    [22] S. Kinreich, A. Djalovski, L. Kraus, Y. Louzoun, R. Feldman, Brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions, Sci. Rep., 7 (2017), 17060. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-17339-5 doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17339-5
    [23] D. M. Ellingsen, A. Duggento, K. Isenburg, C. Jung, J. Lee, J. Gerber, et al., Patient-clinician brain concordance underlies causal dynamics in nonverbal communication and negative affective expressivity, Transl. Psychiatry, 12 (2022), 44. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-022-01810-7 doi: 10.1038/s41398-022-01810-7
    [24] M. Schurz, J. Radua, M. G. Tholen, L. Maliske, D. S. Margulies, R. B. Mars, et al., Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: A neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind, Psychol. Bull., 147 (2021), 293–327. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000303 doi: 10.1037/bul0000303
    [25] L. Ficco, L. Mancuso, J. Manuello, A. Teneggi, D. Liloia, S. Duca, et al., Disentangling predictive processing in the brain: a meta-analytic study in favour of a predictive network, Sci. Rep., 11 (2021), 16258. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-95603-5 doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-95603-5
    [26] G. Rizzolatti, L. Cattaneo, M. Fabbri-Destro, S. Rozzi, Cortical mechanisms underlying the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action understanding, Physiol. Rev., 94 (2014), 655–706. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00009.2013 doi: 10.1152/physrev.00009.2013
    [27] M. Arioli, N. Canessa, Neural processing of social interaction: Coordinate-based meta-analytic evidence from human neuroimaging studies, Hum. Brain Mapp., 40 (2019), 3712–3737. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24627 doi: 10.1002/hbm.24627
    [28] K. Lehmann, D. Bolis, K. J. Friston, L. Schilbach, M. J. D. Ramstead, P. Kanske, An active-inference approach to second-person neuroscience, Perspect. Psychol. Sci., 2023 (2023), 17456916231188000. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916231188000 doi: 10.1177/17456916231188000
    [29] J. Barnby, G. Bellucci, N. Alon, L. Schilbach, V. Bell, C. Frith, et al., Beyond theory of mind: A formal framework for social inference and representation, PsyarXiv, 2023. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/cmgu7
    [30] D. Wei, S. Tsheringla, J. C. McPartland, A. Allsop, Combinatorial approaches for treating neuropsychiatric social impairment, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London, Ser. B, 377 (2022), 20210051. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2021.0051 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0051
    [31] T. Penton, C. Catmur, M. J. Banissy, G. Bird, V. Walsh, Non-invasive stimulation of the social brain: the methodological challenges, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 17 (2022), 15–25. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa102 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa102
    [32] H. K. Kim, D. M. Blumberger, J. Downar, Z. J. Daskalakis, Systematic review of biological markers of therapeutic repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in neurological and psychiatric disorders, Clin. Neurophysiol., 132 (2021), 429–448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2020.11.025 doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.11.025
    [33] A. Czeszumski, S. Eustergerling, A. Lang, D. Menrath, M. Gerstenberger, S. Schuberth, et al., Hyperscanning: A valid method to study neural inter-brain underpinnings of social interaction, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 (2020), 39. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00039 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00039
    [34] A. L. Valencia, T. Froese, What binds us? Inter-brain neural synchronization and its implications for theories of human consciousness, Neurosci. Conscious., 2020 (2020), niaa010. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niaa010 doi: 10.1093/nc/niaa010
    [35] U. Hakim, S. De Felice, P. Pinti, X. Zhang, J. A. Noah, Y. Ono, et al., Quantification of inter-brain coupling: A review of current methods used in haemodynamic and electrophysiological hyperscanning studies, Neuroimage, 280 (2023), 120354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120354 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120354
    [36] A. P. Burgess, On the interpretation of synchronization in EEG hyperscanning studies: a cautionary note, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 7 (2013), 881. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00881 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00881
    [37] G. Dumas, J. Nadel, R. Soussignan, J. Martinerie, L. Garnero, Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction, PLoS One, 5 (2010), e12166. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012166 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012166
    [38] K. Gugnowska, G. Novembre, N. Kohler, A. Villringer, P. E. Keller, D. Sammler, Endogenous sources of interbrain synchrony in duetting pianists, Cereb. Cortex, 32 (2022), 4110–4127. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab469 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab469
    [39] W. Peng, W. Lou, X. Huang, Q. Ye, R. K. Tong, F. Cui, Suffer together, bond together: Brain-to-brain synchronization and mutual affective empathy when sharing painful experiences, Neuroimage, 238 (2021), 118249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118249 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118249
    [40] U. Lindenberger, S. C. Li, W. Gruber, V. Müller, Brains swinging in concert: cortical phase synchronization while playing guitar, BMC Neurosci., 10 (2009), 22. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-10-22 doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-22
    [41] V. Müller, U. Lindenberger, Probing associations between interbrain synchronization and interpersonal action coordination during guitar playing, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1507 (2022), 146–161. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14689 doi: 10.1111/nyas.14689
    [42] L. Astolfi, J. Toppi, A. Ciaramidaro, P. Vogel, C. M. Freitag, M. Siniatchkin, Raising the bar: Can dual scanning improve our understanding of joint action, Neuroimage, 216 (2020), 116813. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116813 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116813
    [43] F. De Vico Fallani, V. Nicosia, R. Sinatra, L. Astolfi, F. Cincotti, D. Mattia, et al., Defecting or not defecting: how to "read" human behavior during cooperative games by EEG measurements, PLoS One, 5 (2010), e14187. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014187 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014187
    [44] L. Astolfi, J. Toppi, F. De Vico Fallani, G. Vecchiato, S. Salinari, D. Mattia, et al., Neuroelectrical hyperscanning measures simultaneous brain activity in humans, Brain Topogr., 23 (2010), 243–256. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-010-0147-9 doi: 10.1007/s10548-010-0147-9
    [45] M. O. Abe, T. Koike, S. Okazaki, S. K. Sugawara, K. Takahashi, K. Watanabe, et al., Neural correlates of online cooperation during joint force production, Neuroimage, 191 (2019), 150–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.003 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.003
    [46] L. Liu, Y. Zhang, Q. Zhou, D. D. Garrett, C. Lu, A. Chen, et al., Auditory-articulatory neural alignment between listener and speaker during verbal communication, Cereb. Cortex, 30 (2020), 942–951. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz138 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz138
    [47] P. Goldstein, I. Weissman-Fogel, G. Dumas, S. G. Shamay-Tsoory, Brain-to-brain coupling during handholding is associated with pain reduction, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 115 (2018), e2528–e2537. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1703643115 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1703643115
    [48] I. Davidesco, E. Laurent, H. Valk, T. West, S. Dikker, C. Milne, et al., Brain-to-brain synchrony predicts long-term memory retention more accurately than individual brain measures, bioRxiv, (2019), 644047. https://doi.org/10.1101/644047 doi: 10.1101/644047
    [49] Y. Tang, X. Liu, C. Wang, M. Cao, S. Deng, X. Du, et al., Different strategies, distinguished cooperation efficiency, and brain synchronization for couples: An fNIRS-based hyperscanning study, Brain Behav., 10 (2020), e01768. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.1768 doi: 10.1002/brb3.1768
    [50] J. Jiang, C. Chen, B. Dai, G. Shi, G. Ding, L. Liu, et al., Leader emergence through interpersonal neural synchronization, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 112 (2015), 4274–4279. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1422930112 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422930112
    [51] Q. Wang, Z. Han, X. Hu, S. Feng, H. Wang, T. Liu, et al., Autism symptoms modulate interpersonal neural synchronization in children with autism spectrum disorder in cooperative interactions, Brain Topogr., 33 (2020), 112–122. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-019-00731-x doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00731-x
    [52] Y. Hu, Y. Hu, X. Li, Y. Pan, X. Cheng, Brain-to-brain synchronization across two persons predicts mutual prosociality, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 12 (2017), 1835–1844. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx118 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx118
    [53] U. Hasson, Y. Nir, I. Levy, G. Fuhrmann, R. Malach, Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision, Science, 303 (2004), 1634–1640. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1089506 doi: 10.1126/science.1089506
    [54] S. A. Nastase, V. Gazzola, U. Hasson, C. Keysers, Measuring shared responses across subjects using intersubject correlation, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 14 (2019), 667–685. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsz037 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz037
    [55] E. Simony, C. J. Honey, J. Chen, O. Lositsky, Y. Yeshurun, A. Wiesel, et al., Dynamic reconfiguration of the default mode network during narrative comprehension, Nat. Commun., 7 (2016), 12141. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms12141 doi: 10.1038/ncomms12141
    [56] J. P. Lachaux, E. Rodriguez, J. Martinerie, F. J. Varela, Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals, Hum. Brain Mapp., 8 (1999), 194–208. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(1999)8:4<194::aid-hbm4>3.0.co;2-c doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(1999)8:4<194::aid-hbm4>3.0.co;2-c
    [57] A. F. C. Hamilton, Hyperscanning: Beyond the hype, Neuron, 109 (2021), 404–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.11.008 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.11.008
    [58] A. Grinsted, J. C. Moore, S. Jevrejeva, Application of the cross wavelet transform and wavelet coherence to geophysical time series, Nonlin. Processes Geophys., 11 (2004), 561–566. https://doi.org/10.5194/npg-11-561-2004 doi: 10.5194/npg-11-561-2004
    [59] L. S. Wang, J. T. Cheng, I. J. Hsu, S. Liou, C. C. Kung, D. Y. Chen, et al., Distinct cerebral coherence in task-based fMRI hyperscanning: cooperation versus competition, Cereb. Cortex, 33 (2022), 421–433. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac075 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac075
    [60] A. K. Seth, A. B. Barrett, L. Barnett, Granger causality analysis in neuroscience and neuroimaging, J. Neurosci., 35 (2015), 3293–3297. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4399-14.2015 doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4399-14.2015
    [61] M. B. Schippers, A. Roebroeck, R. Renken, L. Nanetti, C. Keysers, Mapping the information flow from one brain to another during gestural communication, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 107 (2010), 9388–9393. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001791107 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001791107
    [62] E. Bilek, P. Zeidman, P. Kirsch, H. Tost, A. Meyer-Lindenberg, K. Friston, Directed coupling in multi-brain networks underlies generalized synchrony during social exchange, Neuroimage, 252 (2022), 119038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119038 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119038
    [63] C. B. Holroyd, Interbrain synchrony: on wavy ground, Trends Neurosci., 45 (2022), 346–357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2022.02.002 doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2022.02.002
    [64] Y. Pan, X. Cheng, Two-person approaches to studying social interaction in psychiatry: Uses and clinical relevance, Front. Psychiatry, 11 (2020), 301. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00301 doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00301
    [65] V. Leong, L. Schilbach, The promise of two-person neuroscience for developmental psychiatry: using interaction-based sociometrics to identify disorders of social interaction, Br. J. Psychiatry, 215 (2019), 636–638. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.73 doi: 10.1192/bjp.2019.73
    [66] S. V. Wass, M. Whitehorn, I. Marriott Haresign, E. Phillips, V. Leong, Interpersonal neural entrainment during early social interaction, Trends Cognit. Sci., 24 (2020), 329–342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.006 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.006
    [67] Y. Pan, G. Novembre, B. Song, X. Li, Y. Hu, Interpersonal synchronization of inferior frontal cortices tracks social interactive learning of a song, Neuroimage, 183 (2018), 280–290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.005 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.005
    [68] F. T. Ramseyer, Motion energy analysis (MEA): A primer on the assessment of motion from video, J. Couns. Psychol., 67 (2020), 536–549. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000407 doi: 10.1037/cou0000407
    [69] Z. Cao, G. Hidalgo, T. Simon, S. E. Wei, Y. Sheikh, OpenPose: Realtime multi-person 2D pose estimation using part affinity fields, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell., 43 (2021), 172–186. https://doi.org/10.1109/tpami.2019.2929257 doi: 10.1109/tpami.2019.2929257
    [70] S. Guglielmini, G. Bopp, V. L. Marcar, F. Scholkmann, M. Wolf, Systemic physiology augmented functional near-infrared spectroscopy hyperscanning: a first evaluation investigating entrainment of spontaneous activity of brain and body physiology between subjects, Neurophotonics, 9 (2022), 026601. https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.9.2.026601 doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.9.2.026601
    [71] R. Cañigueral, S. Krishnan-Barman, A. F. C. Hamilton, Social signalling as a framework for second-person neuroscience, Psychon. Bull. Rev., 29 (2022), 2083–2095. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02103-2 doi: 10.3758/s13423-022-02103-2
    [72] L. Kingsbury, S. Huang, J. Wang, K. Gu, P. Golshani, Y. E. Wu, et al., Correlated neural activity and encoding of behavior across brains of socially interacting animals, Cell, 178 (2019), 429–446.e416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.05.022 doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.05.022
    [73] V. Müller, D. Perdikis, M. A. Mende, U. Lindenberger, Interacting brains coming in sync through their minds: an interbrain neurofeedback study, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1500 (2021), 48–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14605 doi: 10.1111/nyas.14605
    [74] L. Duan, W. J. Liu, R. N. Dai, R. Li, C. M. Lu, Y. X. Huang, et al., Cross-brain neurofeedback: scientific concept and experimental platform, PLoS One, 8 (2013), e64590. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064590 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064590
    [75] S. Dikker, G. Michalareas, M. Oostrik, A. Serafimaki, H. M. Kahraman, M. E. Struiksma, et al., Crowdsourcing neuroscience: Inter-brain coupling during face-to-face interactions outside the laboratory, Neuroimage, 227 (2021), 117436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117436 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117436
    [76] M. Hallett, Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the human brain, Nature, 406 (2000), 147–150. https://doi.org/10.1038/35018000 doi: 10.1038/35018000
    [77] J. Vosskuhl, D. Struber, C. S. Herrmann, Non-invasive brain stimulation: A paradigm shift in understanding brain oscillations, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 (2018), 211. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00211 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00211
    [78] A. Liu, M. Vöröslakos, G. Kronberg, S. Henin, M. R. Krause, Y. Huang, et al., Immediate neurophysiological effects of transcranial electrical stimulation, Nat. Commun., 9 (2018), 5092. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-07233-7 doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07233-7
    [79] C. S. Herrmann, M. M. Murray, S. Ionta, A. Hutt, J. Lefebvre, Shaping intrinsic neural oscillations with periodic stimulation, J. Neurosci., 36 (2016), 5328–5337. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0236-16.2016 doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0236-16.2016
    [80] S. Alagapan, S. L. Schmidt, J. Lefebvre, E. Hadar, H. W. Shin, F. Frӧhlich, Modulation of cortical oscillations by low-frequency direct cortical stimulation is state-dependent, PloS Biol., 14 (2016), e1002424. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002424 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002424
    [81] N. Takeuchi, Perspectives on rehabilitation using non-invasive brain stimulation based on second-person neuroscience of teaching-learning interactions, Front. Psychol., 12 (2022), 789637. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.789637 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.789637
    [82] Y. Cabral-Calderin, M. Wilke, Probing the link between perception and oscillations: Lessons from transcranial alternating current stimulation, Neuroscientist, 26 (2020), 57–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858419828646 doi: 10.1177/1073858419828646
    [83] V. Müller, U. Lindenberger, Hyper-brain networks support romantic kissing in humans, PloS One, 9 (2014), e112080. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112080 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112080
    [84] J. Toppi, G. Borghini, M. Petti, E. J. He, V. De Giusti, B. He, et al., Investigating cooperative behavior in ecological settings: An EEG hyperscanning study, PloS One, 11 (2016), e0154236. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0154236 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154236
    [85] V. Leong, E. Byrne, K. Clackson, S. Georgieva, S. Lam, S. Wass, Speaker gaze increases information coupling between infant and adult brains, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 114 (2017), 13290–13295. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1702493114 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1702493114
    [86] Y. Mu, C. Guo, S. Han, Oxytocin enhances inter-brain synchrony during social coordination in male adults, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 11 (2016), 1882–1893. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw106 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw106
    [87] O. A. Heggli, I. Konvalinka, J. Cabral, E. Brattico, M. L. Kringelbach, P. Vuust, Transient brain networks underlying interpersonal strategies during synchronized action, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 16 (2021), 19–30. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa056 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa056
    [88] A. Pérez, M. Carreiras, J. A. Duñabeitia, Brain-to-brain entrainment: EEG interbrain synchronization while speaking and listening, Sci. Rep., 7 (2017), 4190. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-04464-4 doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-04464-4
    [89] J. Sünger, V. Müller, U. Lindenberger, Directionality in hyperbrain networks discriminates between leaders and followers in guitar duets, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 7 (2013), 234. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00234 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00234
    [90] Y. Mu, S. Han, M. J. Gelfand, The role of gamma interbrain synchrony in social coordination when humans face territorial threats, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 12 (2017), 1614–1623. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx093 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx093
    [91] N. Kopell, G. B. Ermentrout, M. A. Whittington, R. D. Traub, Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 97 (2000), 1867–1872. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.4.1867 doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1867
    [92] P. J. Uhlhaas, W. Singer, Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric disorders: toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks, Neuron, 75 (2012), 963–980. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004
    [93] K. J. Friston, T. Parr, Y. Yufik, N. Sajid, C. J. Price, E. Holmes, Generative models, linguistic communication and active inference, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 118 (2020), 42–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.005 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.005
    [94] E. Tognoli, J. A. Kelso, The coordination dynamics of social neuromarkers, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 9 (2015), 563. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00563 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00563
    [95] C. Peylo, Y. Hilla, P. Sauseng, Cause or consequence? Alpha oscillations in visuospatial attention, Trends Neurosci., 44 (2021), 705–713. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2021.05.004 doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2021.05.004
    [96] W. Klimesch, α-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information, Trends Cognit. Sci., 16 (2012), 606–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007
    [97] S. Hoehl, M. Fairhurst, A. Schirmer, Interactional synchrony: signals, mechanisms and benefits, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 16 (2021), 5–18. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa024 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa024
    [98] N. J. Davis, S. P. Tomlinson, H. M. Morgan, The role of beta-frequency neural oscillations in motor control, J. Neurosci., 32 (2012), 403–404. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5106-11.2012 doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5106-11.2012
    [99] B. Pollok, D. Latz, V. Krause, M. Butz, A. Schnitzler, Changes of motor-cortical oscillations associated with motor learning, Neuroscience, 275 (2014), 47–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.008 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.008
    [100] V. Müller, J. Sünger, U. Lindenberger, Intra- and inter-brain synchronization during musical improvisation on the guitar, PloS One, 8 (2013), e73852. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0073852 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073852
    [101] C. S. Herrmann, D. Strüber, R. F. Helfrich, A. K. Engel, EEG oscillations: From correlation to causality, Int. J. Psychophysiol., 103 (2016), 12–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.02.003 doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.02.003
    [102] S. H. Williams, D. Johnston, Kinetic properties of two anatomically distinct excitatory synapses in hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons, J. Neurophysiol., 66 (1991), 1010–1020. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1991.66.3.1010 doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.3.1010
    [103] G. Novembre, G. Knoblich, L. Dunne, P. E. Keller, Interpersonal synchrony enhanced through 20 Hz phase-coupled dual brain stimulation, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 12 (2017), 662–670. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw172 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw172
    [104] C. Szymanski, V. Müller, T. R. Brick, T. von Oertzen, U. Lindenberger, Hyper-transcranial alternating current stimulation: experimental manipulation of inter-brain synchrony, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 11 (2017), 539. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00539 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00539
    [105] Y. Pan, G. Novembre, B. Song, Y. Zhu, Y. Hu, Dual brain stimulation enhances interpersonal learning through spontaneous movement synchrony, Social Cognit. Affective Neurosci., 16 (2021), 210–221. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa080 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa080
    [106] R. T. Canolty, R. T. Knight, The functional role of cross-frequency coupling, Trends Cognit. Sci., 14 (2010), 506–515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.001 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.001
    [107] B. Asamoah, A. Khatoun, M. Mc Laughlin, tACS motor system effects can be caused by transcutaneous stimulation of peripheral nerves, Nat. Commun., 10 (2019), 266. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-08183-w doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-08183-w
    [108] G. Novembre, G. D. Iannetti, Hyperscanning alone cannot prove causality. Multibrain stimulation can, Trends Cognit. Sci., 25 (2021), 96–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.11.003 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.11.003
    [109] S. L. Koole, W. Tschacher, Synchrony in psychotherapy: A review and an integrative framework for the therapeutic alliance, Front. Psychol., 7 (2016), 862. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00862 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00862
    [110] M. Bishop, N. Kayes, K. McPherson, Understanding the therapeutic alliance in stroke rehabilitation, Disability Rehabil., 43 (2021), 1074–1083. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2019.1651909 doi: 10.1080/09638288.2019.1651909
    [111] P. Søndenå, G. Dalusio-King, C. Hebron, Conceptualisation of the therapeutic alliance in physiotherapy: is it adequate, Musculoskeletal Sci. Pract., 46 (2020), 102131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msksp.2020.102131 doi: 10.1016/j.msksp.2020.102131
    [112] P. Mistiaen, M. van Osch, L. van Vliet, J. Howick, F. L. Bishop, Z. Di Blasi, et al., The effect of patient-practitioner communication on pain: a systematic review, Eur. J. Pain, 20 (2016), 675–688. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.797 doi: 10.1002/ejp.797
    [113] L. Schilbach, Towards a second-person neuropsychiatry, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London, Ser. B, 371 (2016), 20150081. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0081 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0081
    [114] L. Schilbach, J. M. Lahnakoski, Clinical neuroscience meets second-person neuropsychiatry, in Social and Affective Neuroscience of Everyday Human Interaction: From Theory to Methodology, Cham (CH): Springer, (2023), 177–191.
    [115] L. E. Quiñones-Camacho, F. A. Fishburn, K. Belardi, D. L. Williams, T. J. Huppert, S. B. Perlman, Dysfunction in interpersonal neural synchronization as a mechanism for social impairment in autism spectrum disorder, Autism Res., 14 (2021), 1585–1596. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2513 doi: 10.1002/aur.2513
    [116] E. Bilek, G. Stößel, A. Schüfer, L. Clement, M. Ruf, L. Robnik, et al., State-dependent cross-brain information flow in borderline personality disorder, JAMA Psychiatry, 74 (2017), 949–957. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.1682 doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.1682
    [117] Y. Zhang, T. Meng, Y. Hou, Y. Pan, Y. Hu, Interpersonal brain synchronization associated with working alliance during psychological counseling. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging, 282 (2018), 103–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.09.007 doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.09.007
    [118] N. Takeuchi, T. Mori, Y. Suzukamo, S. I. Izumi, Integration of teaching processes and learning assessment in the prefrontal cortex during a video game teaching-learning task, Front. Psychol., 7 (2017), 2052. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02052 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02052
    [119] L. Zheng, C. Chen, W. Liu, Y. Long, H. Zhao, X. Bai, et al., Enhancement of teaching outcome through neural prediction of the students' knowledge state, Hum. Brain Mapp., 39 (2018), 3046–3057. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24059 doi: 10.1002/hbm.24059
    [120] L. Zhang, X. Xu, Z. Li, L. Chen, L. Feng, Interpersonal neural synchronization predicting learning outcomes from teaching-learning interaction: A Meta-analysis, Front. Psychol., 13 (2022), 835147. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.835147 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.835147
    [121] S. M. Fleming, R. J. Dolan, The neural basis of metacognitive ability, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London, Ser. B, 367 (2012), 1338–1349. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0417 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0417
    [122] A. G. Vaccaro, S. M. Fleming, Thinking about thinking: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of metacognitive judgements, Brain Neurosci. Adv., 2 (2018), 2398212818810591. https://doi.org/10.1177/2398212818810591 doi: 10.1177/2398212818810591
    [123] J. F. Martín-Rodríguez, J. León-Carrión, Theory of mind deficits in patients with acquired brain injury: a quantitative review, Neuropsychologia, 48 (2010), 1181–1191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.009 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.009
    [124] M. Al Banna, N. A. Redha, F. Abdulla, B. Nair, C. Donnellan, Metacognitive function poststroke: a review of definition and assessment, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 87 (2016), 161–166. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2015-310305 doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2015-310305
    [125] B. Nijsse, J. M. Spikman, J. M. A. Visser-Meily, P. L. M. de Kort, C. M. van Heugten, Social cognition impairments are associated with behavioural changes in the long term after stroke, PloS One, 14 (2019), e0213725. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213725 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213725
    [126] Y. X. Yeo, C. F. Pestell, R. S. Bucks, F. Allanson, M. Weinborn, Metacognitive knowledge and functional outcomes in adults with acquired brain injury: A meta-analysis, Neuropsychol. Rehabil., 31 (2021), 453–478. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2019.1704421 doi: 10.1080/09602011.2019.1704421
    [127] P. Lakatos, J. Gross, G. Thut, A new unifying account of the roles of neuronal entrainment, Curr. Biol., 29 (2019), R890–R905. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.075 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.075
    [128] K. B. Jensen, P. Petrovic, C. E. Kerr, I. Kirsch, J. Raicek, A. Cheetham, et al., Sharing pain and relief: neural correlates of physicians during treatment of patients, Mol. Psychiatry, 19 (2014), 392–398. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2012.195 doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.195
    [129] S. G. Shamay-Tsoory, N. I. Eisenberger, Getting in touch: A neural model of comforting touch, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 130 (2021), 263–273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.08.030 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.08.030
    [130] B. M. Fitzgibbon, M. J. Giummarra, N. Georgiou-Karistianis, P. G. Enticott, J. L. Bradshaw, Shared pain: from empathy to synaesthesia, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 34 (2010), 500–512. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.007 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.007
    [131] D. M. Ellingsen, K. Isenburg, C. Jung, J. Lee, J. Gerber, I. Mawla, et al., Dynamic brain-to-brain concordance and behavioral mirroring as a mechanism of the patient-clinician interaction, Sci. Adv., 6 (2020), eabc1304. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abc1304 doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abc1304
    [132] T. J. Kaptchuk, F. G. Miller, Placebo effects in medicine, N. Engl. J. Med., 373 (2015), 8–9. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp1504023 doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1504023
    [133] M. Ienca, R. W. Kressig, F. Jotterand, B. Elger, Proactive ethical design for neuroengineering, assistive and rehabilitation technologies: the cybathlon lesson, J. Neuroeng. Rehabil., 14 (2017), 115. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-017-0325-z doi: 10.1186/s12984-017-0325-z
    [134] R. Cohen Kadosh, N. Levy, J. O'Shea, N. Shea, J. Savulescu, The neuroethics of non-invasive brain stimulation, Curr. Biol., 22 (2012), R108–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.013 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.013
    [135] S. G. Shamay-Tsoory, Brains that fire together wire together: Interbrain plasticity underlies learning in social interactions, Neuroscientist, 28 (2022), 543–551. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858421996682 doi: 10.1177/1073858421996682
    [136] A. Gramfort, M. Luessi, E. Larson, D. A. Engemann, D. Strohmeier, C. Brodbeck, et al., MNE software for processing MEG and EEG data, Neuroimage, 86 (2014), 446–460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.027 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.027
    [137] R. D. Pascual-Marqui, C. M. Michel, D. Lehmann, Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain, Int. J. Psychophysiol., 18 (1994), 49–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(84)90014-x doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(84)90014-x
    [138] J. Onton, M. Westerfield, J. Townsend, S. Makeig, Imaging human EEG dynamics using independent component analysis, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 30 (2006), 808–822. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.007 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.007
    [139] C. S. Nam, Z. Traylor, M. Chen, X. Jiang, W. Feng, P. Y. Chhatbar, Direct communication between brains: A systematic PRISMA review of brain-to-brain interface, Front. Neurorobot., 15 (2021), 656943. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbot.2021.656943 doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2021.656943
    [140] G. Thut, T. O. Bergmann, F. Fröhlich, S. R. Soekadar, J. S. Brittain, A. Valero-Cabré, et al., Guiding transcranial brain stimulation by EEG/MEG to interact with ongoing brain activity and associated functions: A position paper, Clin. Neurophysiol., 128 (2017), 843–857. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2017.01.003 doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.01.003
    [141] S. Kohli, A. J. Casson, Removal of gross artifacts of transcranial alternating current stimulation in simultaneous EEG monitoring, Sensors (Basel), 19 (2019), 190. https://doi.org/10.3390/s19010190 doi: 10.3390/s19010190
    [142] D. Bolis, J. Balsters, N. Wenderoth, C. Becchio, L. Schilbach, Beyond autism: introducing the dialectical misattunement hypothesis and a Bayesian account of intersubjectivity, Psychopathology, 50 (2017), 355–372. https://doi.org/10.1159/000484353 doi: 10.1159/000484353
    [143] G. Zarubin, C. Gundlach, V. Nikulin, A. Villringer, M. Bogdan, Transient amplitude modulation of alpha-band oscillations by short-time intermittent closed-loop tACS, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 (2020), 366. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00366 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00366
  • Reader Comments
  • © 2024 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(1779) PDF downloads(228) Cited by(0)

Other Articles By Authors

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog