Commentary Special Issues

The Bilingual Cognitive Advantage: No Smoke without Fire?

  • Received: 05 May 2015 Accepted: 11 May 2015 Published: 14 May 2015
  • In a commentary on a review article by Paap and colleagues [1], and a response to that review by Saidi and Ansaldo [2], I examine the key arguments for and against the existence of a ‘bilingual advantage’ in cognitive functions, including the effects of small samples, and of confounding variables in studies on both sides of the debate. While accepting that the behavioural evidence here is inconclusive, I argue that the evidence for wide-ranging, plastic change in the bilingual brain would seem to predict that bilingualism may have similarly wide-ranging effects on behaviour. Finally, I note that bilingual cognitive advantages - if any exist - are inherently longitudinal phenomena, in the sense that they are thought to emerge as a function of the transfer of practice effects from linguistic to non-linguistic cognitive control skills. In that context, the most direct way to characterise those advantages, and the mechanisms that make them possible, may be with longitudinal studies, which also naturally control for many of the factors that may confound the cross-sectional studies which have dominated the field so far.

    Citation: Thomas M. H. Hope. The Bilingual Cognitive Advantage: No Smoke without Fire?[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(2): 58-65. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.2.58

    Related Papers:

  • In a commentary on a review article by Paap and colleagues [1], and a response to that review by Saidi and Ansaldo [2], I examine the key arguments for and against the existence of a ‘bilingual advantage’ in cognitive functions, including the effects of small samples, and of confounding variables in studies on both sides of the debate. While accepting that the behavioural evidence here is inconclusive, I argue that the evidence for wide-ranging, plastic change in the bilingual brain would seem to predict that bilingualism may have similarly wide-ranging effects on behaviour. Finally, I note that bilingual cognitive advantages - if any exist - are inherently longitudinal phenomena, in the sense that they are thought to emerge as a function of the transfer of practice effects from linguistic to non-linguistic cognitive control skills. In that context, the most direct way to characterise those advantages, and the mechanisms that make them possible, may be with longitudinal studies, which also naturally control for many of the factors that may confound the cross-sectional studies which have dominated the field so far.


    加载中
    [1] Paap KR, Sawi OM, Dalibar C, et al. (2014) The Brain Mechanisms Underlying the Cognitive Benefits of Bilingualism may be Extraordinarily Difficult to Discover. AIMS neurosci 1: 245-256. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.3.245
    [2] Saidi LG, Ansaldo AI (2015) Can a Second Language Help You in More Ways Than One? AIMS neurosci 1: 52-57.
    [3] Duyck W, Diependaele K, Drieghe D, et al. (2004) The Size of the Cross-Lingua Masked Phonological Priming Effect Does Not Depend on Second Language Proficiency. Exp Psychol 51: 116-124. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.51.2.116
    [4] Lalor E, Kirsner K (2000) Cross-lingual transfer effects between English and Italian cognates and noncognates. Int J Biling 4: 385-398. doi: 10.1177/13670069000040030501
    [5] Lemhöfer K, Dijkstra T, Michel M (2004) Three languages, one ECHO: Cognate effects in trilingual word recognition. Lang Cogn Process 19: 585-611. doi: 10.1080/01690960444000007
    [6] Kroll JF (2008) Juggling two languages in one mind. Psychological Science Agenda, American Psychological Association 22.
    [7] Paap KR, Johnson HA, Sawi O (2014) Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? J CognPsychol 26: 615-639.
    [8] Green DW (2008) Bilingual Aphasia: Adapted Language Networks and their Control. Annu Rev Appl Linguist 28: 25-48. doi: 10.1017/S0267190508080057
    [9] Green DW, Abutalebi J (2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. J Cogn Psychol 25: 515-530. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
    [10] Bialystok E, Majumder S, Martin MM (2003) Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Appl Psycholinguistics 24: 27-44.
    [11] Bialystok E, Martin MM (2004) Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental science 7: 325-339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.x
    [12] Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Freedman M (2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia 45: 459-464. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.009
    [13] Fernandes MA, Craik F, Bialystok E, et al. (2007) Effects of bilingualism, aging, and semantic relatedness on memory under divided attention. Can J Exp Psychol 61: 128-141. doi: 10.1037/cjep2007014
    [14] Bialystok E, Craik F, Luk G (2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 34: 859-873. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859
    [15] Sterne JA, Davey Smith G (2001) Sifting the evidence-what's wrong with significance tests? Bmj 322: 226-231. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7280.226
    [16] Button KS, Ioannidis JPA, Mokrysz C, et al. (2013) Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 14: 365-376. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475
    [17] Hilchey MD, Klein RM (2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychon Bull Rev 18: 625-658.
    [18] Anton E, Dunabeitia JA, Estevez A, et al. (2014) Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Front Psychol 5: 398.
    [19] Dunabeitia JA, Hernandez JA, Anton E, et al. (2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality? Exp Psychol 61: 234-251. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000243
    [20] Paap KR, Greenberg ZI (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cogn Psychol 66: 232-258. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
    [21] Paap KR, Sawi O (2014) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Front Psychol 5: 962.
    [22] Morton JB, Harper SN (2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Dev Sci 10: 719-726.
    [23] Kirk NW, Fiala L, Scott-Brown KC, et al. (2014) No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. J Cogn Psychol (Hove) 26: 640-648. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.929580
    [24] Calvo A, Bialystok E (2014) Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition 130: 278-288. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015
    [25] Ljungberg JK, Hansson P, Andrés P, et al. (2013) A Longitudinal Study of Memory Advantages in Bilinguals. PLoS ONE 8: e73029. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073029
    [26] Sheng L, Lu Y, Gollan TH (2014) Assessing language dominance in Mandarin-English bilinguals: Convergence and divergence between subjective and objective measures. Biling (Camb Engl) 17: 364-383. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000424
    [27] Ratcliff R, McKoon G (2008) The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks. Neural comput 20: 873-922. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420
    [28] Luk G, Anderson JAE, Craik FIM, et al. (2011) Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain Cogn 74: 347-357.
    [29] Hunt LT, Kolling N, Soltani A, et al. (2012) Mechanisms underlying cortical activity during value-guided choice. Nat Neurosci 15: 470-476. doi: 10.1038/nn.3017
    [30] Abutalebi J, Della Rosa PA, Green DW, et al. (2012) Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cereb Cortex 22: 2076-2086. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr287
    [31] Shenhav A, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD (2013) The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79: 217-240. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007
    [32] di Pellegrino G, Ciaramelli E, Ladavas E (2007) The regulation of cognitive control following rostral anterior cingulate cortex lesion in humans. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 275-286. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.2.275
    [33] Glascher J, Adolphs R, Damasio H, et al. (2012) Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109: 14681-14686. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206608109
    [34] Fellows LK, Farah MJ (2005) Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? Brain 128: 788-796. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh405
    [35] Abutalebi J, Della Rosa PA, Green DW, et al. (2012) Bilingualism Tunes the Anterior Cingulate Cortex for Conflict Monitoring. Cereb Cortex 22: 2076-2086. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr287
    [36] Mechelli A, Crinion JT, Noppeney U, et al. (2004) Neurolinguistics: Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature 431: 757-757. doi: 10.1038/431757a
    [37] Mårtensson J, Eriksson J, Bodammer NC, et al. (2012) Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. NeuroImage 63: 240-244. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.043
    [38] Kovelman I, Baker SA, Petitto L-A (2008) Bilingual and Monolingual Brains Compared: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Investigation of Syntactic Processing and a Possible “Neural Signature” of Bilingualism. J cogn neurosci20: 153-169. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20011
    [39] Olsen RK, Pangelinan MM, Bogulski C, et al. (2015) The effect of lifelong bilingualism on regional grey and white matter volume. Brain Res.
    [40] Moreno S, Bialystok E, Wodniecka Z, et al. (2010) Conflict Resolution in Sentence Processing by Bilinguals. J Neurolinguistics 23: 564-579. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.05.002
    [41] Calabria M, Hernandez M, Branzi FM, et al. (2012) Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: evidence from task switching. Front Psychol 2: 399.
    [42] Sagi D (2011) Perceptual learning in Vision Research. Vision Research 51: 1552-1566. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.019
    [43] Webster J, Whitworth A, Morris J (2015) Is it time to stop “fishing”? A review of generalisation following aphasia intervention. Aphasiology: 1-25.
    [44] Welch DB, Seitz AR (2013) Processing Irrelevant Location Information: Practice and Transfer Effects in a Simon Task. PLoS ONE 8: e64993. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064993
    [45] Macnamara BN, Conway AR (2014) Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage: influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory. Psychon Bull Rev 21: 520-525. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0524-y
    [46] Prior A, Gollan TH (2013) The elusive link between language control and executive control: A case of limited transfer. J Cogn Psychol (Hove) 25: 622-645. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.821993
  • Reader Comments
  • © 2015 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(5454) PDF downloads(1328) Cited by(7)

Article outline

Other Articles By Authors

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog