Mini review

The effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on food craving and food intake in individuals affected by obesity and overweight: a mini review of the magnitude of the effects

  • Received: 15 March 2022 Revised: 14 July 2022 Accepted: 20 July 2022 Published: 01 August 2022
  • Obesity represents one of the wellness diseases concurring to increase the incidence of diabetes, cardiovascular diseases, and cancer. One of the main perpetuating factors of obesity is food craving, which is characterized by an urgent desire to eat a large and various amount of food, regardless of calories requirement or satiety signals, and it might be addressed to the alteration of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activity. Despite most of the gold-standard therapies focus on symptom treatment only, non-invasive brain stimulation techniques such as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) could help treat overeating by modulating specific neural pathways. The current systematic review was conducted to identify whether convergent evidence supporting the usefulness of tDCS to deal with food craving are present in the literature. The review was conducted by searching articles published up to January 1st 2022 on MEDLINE, Scopus and PsycInfo databases. We included studies investigating the effects of tDCS on food craving in subjects affected by overweight and obesity. According to eligibility criteria, 5 articles were included. Results showed that tDCS targeting left DLPFC with unipolar montage induced ameliorating effects on food craving. Controversial results were shown for the other studies, that might be ascribable to the use of bipolar montage, and the choice of other target areas. Further investigations including expectancy effect control, larger sample sizes and follow-up are needed to support more robust conclusions. To conclude, tDCS combined with the use of psychoeducative intervention, diet and physical activity, might represents a potential to manage food craving in individuals with overweight and obesity.

    Citation: Graziella Orrù, Valentina Cesari, Eleonora Malloggi, Ciro Conversano, Danilo Menicucci, Alessandro Rotondo, Cristina Scarpazza, Laura Marchi, Angelo Gemignani. The effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on food craving and food intake in individuals affected by obesity and overweight: a mini review of the magnitude of the effects[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(3): 358-372. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022020

    Related Papers:

    [1] Graziella Orrù, Marina Baroni, Ciro Conversano, Angelo Gemignani . Exploring the therapeutic potential of tDCS, TMS and DBS in overcoming tobacco use disorder: an umbrella review. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(4): 449-467. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024027
    [2] Priscila Centeno Crespo, Leo Anderson Meira Martins, Otávio Garcia Martins, Clara Camacho Dos Reis, Ricardo Netto Goulart, Andressa de Souza, Liciane Fernandes Medeiros, Vanessa Leal Scarabelot, Giovana Duzzo Gamaro, Sabrina Pereira Silva, Marcos Roberto de Oliveira, Iraci Lucena da Silva Torres, Izabel Cristina Custódio de Souza . Short-term effectiveness of transcranial direct current stimulation in the nociceptive behavior of neuropathic pain rats in development. AIMS Neuroscience, 2023, 10(4): 433-446. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2023032
    [3] Mirela Dubravac, Beat Meier . Stimulating the parietal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): no effects on attention and memory. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(1): 33-46. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021002
    [4] Ubaid Ansari, Jimmy Wen, Isabel Taguinod, Dawnica Nadora, Denise Nadora, Forshing Lui . Exploring dietary approaches in the prevention and management of Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis: A literature review. AIMS Neuroscience, 2023, 10(4): 376-387. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2023028
    [5] Marian E. Berryhill . Longitudinal tDCS: Consistency across Working Memory Training Studies. AIMS Neuroscience, 2017, 4(2): 71-86. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.2.71
    [6] Fiammetta Iannuzzo, Silvia Crudo, Gianpaolo Antonio Basile, Fortunato Battaglia, Carmenrita Infortuna, Maria Rosaria Anna Muscatello, Antonio Bruno . Efficacy and safety of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques for the treatment of nicotine addiction: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(3): 212-225. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024014
    [7] Kiran Kumar Siddappaji, Shubha Gopal . Molecular mechanisms in Alzheimer's disease and the impact of physical exercise with advancements in therapeutic approaches. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(3): 357-389. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021020
    [8] Ubaid Ansari, Meraj Alam, Dawnica Nadora, Zohaer Muttalib, Vincent Chen, Isabel Taguinod, Megan FitzPatrick, Jimmy Wen, Zaid Ansari, Forshing Lui . Assessing the efficacy of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis drugs in slowing disease progression: A literature review. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(2): 166-177. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024010
    [9] Valentina Cesari, Graziella Orrù, Andrea Piarulli, Alessandra Vallefuoco, Franca Melfi, Angelo Gemignani, Danilo Menicucci . The effects of right temporoparietal junction stimulation on embodiment, presence, and performance in teleoperation. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(3): 352-373. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024022
    [10] Marie Vandekerckhove, Yu-lin Wang . Emotion, emotion regulation and sleep: An intimate relationship. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(1): 1-17. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.1.1
  • Obesity represents one of the wellness diseases concurring to increase the incidence of diabetes, cardiovascular diseases, and cancer. One of the main perpetuating factors of obesity is food craving, which is characterized by an urgent desire to eat a large and various amount of food, regardless of calories requirement or satiety signals, and it might be addressed to the alteration of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activity. Despite most of the gold-standard therapies focus on symptom treatment only, non-invasive brain stimulation techniques such as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) could help treat overeating by modulating specific neural pathways. The current systematic review was conducted to identify whether convergent evidence supporting the usefulness of tDCS to deal with food craving are present in the literature. The review was conducted by searching articles published up to January 1st 2022 on MEDLINE, Scopus and PsycInfo databases. We included studies investigating the effects of tDCS on food craving in subjects affected by overweight and obesity. According to eligibility criteria, 5 articles were included. Results showed that tDCS targeting left DLPFC with unipolar montage induced ameliorating effects on food craving. Controversial results were shown for the other studies, that might be ascribable to the use of bipolar montage, and the choice of other target areas. Further investigations including expectancy effect control, larger sample sizes and follow-up are needed to support more robust conclusions. To conclude, tDCS combined with the use of psychoeducative intervention, diet and physical activity, might represents a potential to manage food craving in individuals with overweight and obesity.


    Abbreviations

    atDCS:

    anodal transcranial direct current stimulation; 

    BMI:

    Body Mass Index; 

    ctDCS:

    cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation; 

    DLPFC:

    dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; 

    lDLPFC:

    left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; 

    PRISMA:

    Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review; 

    RCTs:

    randomized controlled trials; 

    rDLPFC:

    right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; 

    tDCS:

    transcranial direct current stimulation; 

    VAS:

    visual analogue scale

    Over the last few decades, obesity has become one of the healthcare-related burdens of the Western society. Indeed, 11% of the European population aged between 18 and 69 and 14% of individuals aged over 65 are affected by this medical condition [1]. Moreover, obesity increases the incidence of diabetes, cardiovascular diseases, and cancer, thus augmenting the cost of the healthcare system, especially for professionals [2]; indeed, among individuals affected by obesity, 31% suffer from hypertension, 8% from diabetes and 8% from respiratory diseases [1]. The prevalence of this condition was particularly exacerbated by COVID-19 quarantine, during which dysfunctional conducts intimately linked to stressful situations such as food craving have dramatically increased [3][5]. Food craving is characterized by two intrinsic key-factors: desire intensity for specific food and specificity for certain type of food [6],[7]. Food craving, overeating and erratic alimentary regimen are some of the perpetuating factors of obesity, that can also be ascribable to failure in impulsive behaviour control [8] and dysregulation of many cognitive domains (i.e., decision-making, risk-taking, and working memory). Indeed, this cognitive dysregulation have bidirectionally been associated with obesity and overweight conditions [9] due to the lack of adherence to dietary restrictions and promoting snacking behaviours [10]. Hunger and food intake are regulated by different hierarchical processes that rely on different but intertwined neural circuitries, such as homeostatic and reward circuits, and cognitive areas [11]; in particular, a key area which is primarily involved in modulating impulsiveness and overconsumption is the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [11], whose decreased activation has been found in patients with obesity [12], hence preventing the modification of lifestyle habits (e.g., food intake and physical activity) [13]. An impairment in the prefrontal cortical inhibitory networks could be considered as one of the markers of pathophysiology of impulsive behaviour, since the prefrontal cortex plays a pivotal role in decision making and in gating automatic responses [14],[15].

    In the context of treatments, tDCS has been revealed to be a promising tool and extensively used in a wide variety of psychiatric and neurological disorders [16][19], including food-related dysfunctional behaviour [20]. tDCS involves the modulation of cortical excitability by means of a constant low-amperage electrical current (~ 1–2 milliampere, mA) to the cortex via scalp electrodes; cortical excitability is increased by anodic stimulation and decreased by cathodic one [21],[22]. Given the role of the DLPFC in impulsive behaviour control [23],[24], the application of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques has mainly focused on this brain area. In this line, a recent systematic review stated the potential of tDCS on response inhibition, with anodal tDCS over right prefrontal cortex enhancing this cognitive process [25]: this represents an optimal rationale to introduce the use of tDCS in the field of obesity and overweight, given the presence of impulsive behavior and lack of response inhibition.

    Since one of the latest reviews on tDCS effects on food craving did not report significant results [26], and this might be ascribable to the inclusion of mixed samples studies, and since the study performed by Mostafavi et al. [27] reported positive effects that might be biased by the inclusion of studies that applied other interventions (i.e., diet, physical exercise) in addition to tDCS, a literature update focusing on a more homogeneous sample and on the mere effect of tDCS is needed. When studying food craving concomitant with other eating disorders such as binge eating and bulimia, results could be biased by the apparent common phenotype of food craving and eating disorder; even though people affected by binge eating and bulimia displayed higher food craving if compared to non-clinical sample [28], there is no compelling evidence that these differences reflect higher reactivity to food cues in terms of larger increases in food craving intensity in response to food cues (which would explain their difficulties in controlling food intake [29].

    Our review will focus on the neuro-modulating effects of tDCS on specific brain regions involved in food craving in individuals affected by overweight and obesity.

    The method used for this systematic review satisfies the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis guidelines (PRISMA), which comprises a checklist to ensure the quality of systematic reviews [30], which is fully reported in the supplementary Materials (Table S1). In order to perform an effective search strategy, we adopted the Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcomes and Study Design (PICOS) strategy [31] (Table S2).

    Our search was conducted up to January 1st 2022, and comprised three main phases: (i) identification: a literature search based on queries on the electronic MEDLINE, Scopus, PsycInfo, databases. Boolean operators “AND” and “OR” were applied to combine the following list of keywords related to tDCS and the keywords related to food craving: “transcranial direct current stimulation” AND “hyperphagia” OR “obesity” OR “overweight” OR “food craving” OR “food intake”; (ii) Screening: a manual screening of the articles yielded during the first phase, by evaluating title and abstract; (iii) Eligibility: a more in-depth assessment of the remaining papers based on full-text reading.

    Only articles in English were included. In addition, we also scanned the references list reported by each study. The selected studies (Figure 1) satisfied the preferred reporting items for systematic Review (PRISMA) [30]. During the selection phase studies were included if the following criteria were met: (1) randomized controlled trials (RCTs); (2) individuals over 18 years old; (3) subjects with overweight (body mass index, BMI, between 25 kg/m2 and 29.99 kg/m2) or obesity (BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2); (5) outcomes for food craving and/or food intake was present; (7) articles published in international peer-reviewed and indexed journals; (8) trials examining tDCS efficacy with sham or control condition; (9) studies including tDCS as the only intervention applied. Studies were excluded if (1) participants were diagnosed with eating disorder or were healthy participants with food craving traits; (2) no sham tDCS (3) protocols with treatments in addition to tDCS; (4) no comparisons between conditions; (5) no food craving assessment; (6) no randomized-controlled, placebo-controlled trials and blinding procedure. Articles retrieved were merged into Mendeley database [32] and duplicates were automatically removed using desktop Mendeley reference manager. For each included study, we computed the effect size of significant results.

    To assess the quality of evidence of the studies, we used the modified Jadad scale [33]. The scales ranges from 0 to 8, and points are awarded if the study meets the following qualitative criteria: is described as randomized, 1 point; has appropriate randomization method, 1 point; is described as subject-blinded, 1 point; is described as evaluator-blinded, 1 point; and has description of withdrawals and dropouts, 1 point; presented the inclusion/exclusion criteria, 1 point; described the adverse effects, 1 point; and described statistical analysis, 1 point. The total score for each article was computed by summing the score of each item. Studies with a modified Jadad score ≤ 3 were low-quality randomized controlled trials (RCTs); studies with a modified Jadad score ≥ 4 were considered to be high-quality RCTs. Results of quality assessment are shown in the supplementary materials (Table S3). Also, we used the Cochrane Collaboration's “Risk of Bias” tool to assess bias in randomized controlled studies [34], evaluating the following domains: sequence generation (selection bias), allocation sequence concealment (selection bias), blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias), blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias), incomplete outcome data (attrition bias), selective outcome reporting (reporting bias) and other potential sources of bias. This tool assigns three different score to each analyzed subdomain: low risk, unclear risk, high risk.

    Two independent reviewers (E.M. and V.C.) and one referee (G.O.) were involved in all the above-mentioned processes (data sources and search strategy, procedure for studies selection, quality assessment and data extraction). Any disagreements were resolved through consensus of all authors.

    A search of the databases disclosed 468 articles. After duplicates removal, 351 records were screened. After the screening phase, 20 full-text articles were retained. Among these, 14 studies were excluded for the following reasons: 5 of them included other treatments in addition to tDCS; 3 of them did not include food craving/food intake outcomes; 3 studies recruited healthy subjects; 2 studies had abstract only; one study did not perform a randomized design; one study did not perform tDCS sham session. Finally, 5 studies were included (Figure 1).

    Figure 1.  Flow chart of the studies selection.

    The included studies diverged in sample size dimensions, ranging from a minimum of 9 participants to a maximum of 74 participants. There was a wide sex heterogeneity in the composition of the samples. Four studies included both females and males, while 1 study recruited female participants only [35].

    Regarding the number of sessions, 2 studies [35],[36] used a single-session protocol, one study [37] performed 2 sessions, one study employed 3 and 6 sessions [38] and one study used 15 sessions [39].

    Concerning session duration, 3 studies stimulated for 20 minutes [35][37], and 2 studies for 40 minutes [38],[39].

    As regards the target area, 2 studies applied a unipolar montage: one of them first administered anodal tDCS (atDCS) and then cathodal tDCS (ctDCS) over the left DLPFC (lDLPFC) [38] and the other one applied anodal tDCS over the lDLPFC [39]; the other 3 studies applied a bipolar montage: two of them stimulated the lDLPFC and the other one stimulated the right DLPFC (rDLPFC) [37].

    All the selected studies were randomized and sham controlled, according to inclusion criteria. Concerning the blinding procedure, 2 studies were single blind [37],[39], 2 studies were double blind [35],[38] and one study had a mixed blinding procedure [36]. Regarding the study design, 2 studies were crossover [35],[37], 3 studies were between subjects [36],[38],[39].

    According to Jadad's score [33], 4 studies out of 5 were high-quality RCTs (score ≥ 4) and one study was classified as nearly high-quality RCTs (score = 3.5). The average between-studies score was 5.5 ± 1.45 (Table S3). For what concern Cochrane Collaboration's “Risk of Bias [34], we reported the labels indicating the risk of bias using different colors (Table S4). The subdomain that resulted to be more at risk is the attrition bias, that indicates incomplete outcome data.

    The effect size was computed or retrieved from the selected studies to quantify the magnitude of the experimental effect of significance for food craving measures. Since one study reported no significant results [35], we retrieved/computed effect size for a total of 4 studies out of 5. As regards the magnitude of effect size, one study reported a small effect size for food craving parameters—Cohen's d < 0.5 [38]; 2 studies reported an intermediate effect size – 0.5 ≤ Cohen's d < 0.8; 0.060 ≤ partial η2 < 0.11 [36],[37]; 3 studies displayed a large effect size (Cohen's d ≥ 0.8; partial η2 ≥ 0.11) [37][39]. To avoid overestimation or underestimation of the results, we stratified effect sizes according to their magnitude (i.e., for each study, different effect sizes belonging to small, medium, or large magnitude might have been reported). The effect sizes were calculated and interpreted using psychometrica software [40], and are reported in Table 1.

    Table 1.  tDCS parameters and outcomes.
    Reference Sample Study design Stimulation location Current intensity/electrode size/duration/ follow-up Outcome measures Clinical outcomes and Effect size calculation
    Heinitz et al., 2013 [39] N = 22 subjects with obesity
    active: n = 9
    sham: n = 13
    Single-blinded, randomized, parallel design study a: left DLPFC
    rc: right supraorbital region
    2mA;35 cm2; 40 min; 15 sessions, FU: no Food preferences assessed by SFTTs after vending machine food ad libitum paradigm; VAS for appetite -Decrease in VAS ratings for hunger (p = 0.01; Cohen's d = 1.149) and urge to eat (p = 0.05; Cohen's d = 0.837) in active group compared to sham group
    -Decrease of total energy intake during SFTT relatively lower in satiated individuals (p = 0.01; Cohen's d = 1.183)
    Gluck et al. 2015 [38] N = 9
    subjects with obesity
    active: n = 5
    sham: n = 4
    Randomized, double-blinded, sham-controlled parallel study Study 1
    c: left DLPFC (F3)
    ra: left forearm
    Study 2
    a: left DLPFC (F3)
    rc: right eye
    Study 1:
    2 mA; 25 cm2; 40 min; 3 sessions; FU: no
    Study 2:
    2 mA; 25 cm2; 40 min; 6 sessions; FU: no
    Energy intake and weight change following 3-days ad libitum intake using of an automated vending machines with preferred food -Reduced consumption of kilocalories per day (p = 0.07), especially for fat food (p = 0.03, Cohen's d = 0.309) and soda (p = 0.02, Cohen's d = 0.334) after atDCS versus ctDCS
    -Increased % of weight loss (p = 0.009, Cohen's d = 1.09) after atDCS versus ctDCS.
    Grundeis et al., 2017 [35] N = 25 women with obesity Double-blinded, randomized, sham-controlled, within-subject crossover design Condition 1:
    a: left DLPFC;
    c: right frontal operculum
    Condition 2:
    c: left DLPFC
    a: right frontal operculum
    2 mA, 35 cm2; 20 min; single session Self-rated task performed after food picture task; VAS for appetite (baseline, after tDCS and after ad libitum real food buffet) No effects of atDCS/ctDCS in modulating the desire of visually presented food and calorie intake
    Marron et al., 2019 [37] N = 12
    subjects with obesity
    Single-blinded, randomized, sham-controlled, crossover study a: left DLPFC
    c: right posterior cerebellar lobe
    2 mA; 25 cm2; 20 min; 2 sessions; FU: no Food-related cognitive performance (Food-modified N-back task); general effect of motor performance and working memory (Finger tapping task and digit span test); VAS for appetite -Increased hunger and desire to eat after food exposure in active tDCS and the opposite in sham condition (trend, p = 0.094)
    -Increased hunger in the active condition compared to baseline (p = 0.019; Cohen's d = -0.79)
    -Effect of time in desire to eat, indicating an increase for all participants (p = 0.033, ηp2 = 0.349)
    -Increased number of errors in N-back task after active tDCS (trend, p = 0.085)
    -Improved backward digit span in sham vs active (p = 0.039; Cohen's d = -0.67)
    Ray et al., 2019 [36] N = 74 subjects with obesity and overweight
    told sham/ got sham: n = 18
    told sham /got active: n = 19
    told active/got sham: n = 17
    told active/ got active: n = 20
    Not blind/single blind, randomized design, between-subjects a: right DLPFC
    c: left DLPFC
    2 mA; 24 cm2, single session, 20 min; FU: no Electronic baseline surveys; Food craving task with highly palatable food; eating task with available real food; hunger assessment -No main effect of real vs sham tDCS on craving or eating
    -No interaction effect between tDCS condition and expectation
    -Less craving and eating in participants who were told receiving active tDCS (p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.09)

    Notes: a, anodal; atDCS; ctDCS, cathodal tDCS; d, Cohen's effect size; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; FU, follow up; min, minutes; mA, milliampere; ra, reference anode; rc, reference cathode; SFTTs, snack food taste tests; tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; VAS, Visual Analogue Scale; ηp2, partial eta square

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    To investigate possible changes associated with food consumption, Heinitz and colleagues [39] applied short-term and long-term anodal tDCS targeting the lDLPFC. Findings showed that short-term tDCS did not influence ad libitum food consumption from the vending machines and no weight changes were detected after 4 weeks. However, after 6 weeks, participants showed decreased appetite (p = 0.01) and desire to eat (p = 0.05) compared to the sham condition. After long-term active tDCS, satiated subjects displayed less food intake (p = 0.01) compared to the sham group. The same target area was also chosen by Gluck et al. [38] in the two following protocols: a) ctDCS over the lDLPFC or sham tDCS (study 1); and b) atDCS over the lDLPFC or sham (study 2); participants were exposed to automatic vending machines with their preferred food on three consecutive mornings in both studies. Based on their results, participants showed an overall reduction of calories intake (p = 0.07) per day, accompanied by a significant weight loss (p = 0.02) after atDCS compared to ctDCS.

    No significant effects were detected by Grundeis and colleagues [35], neither after atDCS over the lDLPFC, nor after ctDCS over the lDLPFC on food intake and desire generated by food pictures.

    Another study investigating atDCS and ctDCS over the DLPFC was carried out by Ray et al. [36]. In their study, atDCS and ctDCS were delivered over the rDLPFC and lDLPFC, respectively. Only two groups out of four were blinded for tDCS application (sham/active condition). One group was told it would receive sham tDCS and instead it underwent active tDCS, while the other blind group was told the opposite. Participants who were told they would receive active tDCS reported decreased craving and eating (p < 0.01), compared to those who were told they would receive sham, regardless of real condition.

    Differently from the aforementioned studies, Marron and colleagues [37] explored the effects of modulating DLPFC-cerebellum interactions by applying a new two-session tDCS paradigm on subjects with obesity. The study showed a nearly significant decrease of hunger and desire to eat after sham tDCS measured by VAS (p = 0.094), whereas active condition did not (p = 0.903). Additionally, after active tDCS, subjects showed an increased number of errors during the food-modified working memory task compared to sham condition. Participants also displayed higher scores in the working memory task (Backward Digit Span Test) after the second session in both conditions; sham condition improved backward digit span compared to the active condition.

    This review provided a qualitative synthesis of the effects of tDCS on food craving in subjects affected by obesity and overweight, taking into account several tDCS parameters (i.e., targeted areas, number of sessions), and the magnitude of the experimental effects.

    All studies targeted DLPFC, but differences were detected for the lateralization of the stimulation site and montages (e.g., extracephalic or cephalic return). DLPFC was the sole target area in all the analyzed studies, and this is in line with the pivotal role of DLPFC in executive functions (e.g., planning, decision making, response inhibition and goal-directed behaviour) [41].

    Coherently with the hypothesis explaining the pathogenetic mechanism that might sustain food craving behaviour in obesity, the findings of the current work have been reported according to DLPFC lateralization. According to the right brain hypothesis of obesity [42] postulating rDLPFC lateralization for hunger and eating behaviour, the enhancement of its activity could strengthen inhibitory control by orexigenic areas to suppress hunger in overeating subjects. The pivotal role of the rDLPFC is consistent with recent findings showing altered between-network connectivity in the basal ganglia and rDLPFC in subjects predisposed to obesity, that might reflect a tendency towards habitual behaviour rather than goal-directed behaviour in these individuals [41].

    The second hypothesis arose from studies that reported decreased grey matter density in the lDLPFC [43] and lower activation in the same area after food intake in subjects with obesity. as compared to subjects with BMI within normative range in resting state condition [11],[44]. Ray and colleagues [36] applied atDCS over the rDLPFC and ctDCS over the lDLPFC, but the reduction of food craving was mainly sustained by placebo effect, indicating that expectancy effect was more powerful than the main effect of tDCS itself. In fact, since placebo effect derives from subjective interpretation of context information and most of medical treatments benefits are caused by the brain's response to the treatment context [45], the sole sight of tDCS montage might have acted as contextual cue inducing an expectancy effect in subjects, and it is possible to argue that tDCS might elicit strong behavioural effects regardless of the condition [46]. A recent study found that current intensities conventionally used in non-invasive brain stimulation techniques studies are likely to be insufficient to affect neuronal circuits in a direct way, suggesting that the reported behavioural and cognitive effects may result from indirect mechanisms [41].

    The role of lDLPFC has been reported to be essential also for dietary self-control. In fact, during decisions about food preference, subjects with higher self-control displayed increased activity in this area. Therefore, differences in DLPFC activity, particularly the lDLPFC, may explain individual differences in dietary choices, “vis-à-vis the connection between the DLPFC and inhibitory control[27]. Differently, Grundeis and colleagues [35] applied atDCS over the lDLPFC and ctDCS over the right frontal operculum and vice versa, due to its involvement in gustatory processes [47] and its higher activation during food desire regulation in subjects affected by obesity [48]. Marron and colleagues [37] stimulated the lDLPFC with anode and the right posterior cerebellar lobe with cathode, as the cerebellum is one of the targets of leptin hormone [49] and it is activated by food cues [50]. These last two studies did not find the expected results, and Marron and colleagues [37] even reported an increase in hunger after active tDCS. As explained by authors, this unexpected result may originate from a functional decoupling between the lDLPFC and the cerebellum. In addition, the correlation between decreased activity in the cerebellum and increased hunger is consistent with the inverse relationship between cerebellar integrity and BMI [51].

    As regards the comparison between tDCS parameters, the studies performed by Gluck et al. [38] and Grundeis et al. [35] adopted different montages and polarities to detect differences in food craving outcomes. Gluck et al. [38] found that atDCS exerted a significant ameliorating effect as compared to ctDCS. These differences might be the result of neuronal depolarization induced by anodal stimulation. Furthermore, it is worth noting that Grundeis et al. [35] recruited female participants with obesity, and they did not inquire the menstrual cycle; in this context, the menstrual cycle has been reported to be likely to affect food perception and intake [52].

    Considering the montage, Gluck et al. [38] and Heinitz et al. [39] found out a decrease in food craving, and it could be suggested that the use of extracephalic reference cathode might exert a deeper stimulation allowing a more powerful effect. This is also in line with the aforementioned theory of the lDLPFC role in regulating food craving [11],[44].

    Furthermore, tDCS montage could have acted as a confounding variable, since tDCS induces its highest and longest-lasting EEG changes when delivered bipolarly with the anode on the left and the cathode on the right prefrontal cortex, and unipolarly with the anode on the right prefrontal cortex [53]. The depth of stimulation when using extracephalic cathode location is substantially greater than that of the traditional cephalic arrangement. This would indicate that a higher percentage of the desired stimulation area would be covered through extracephalic locations. Investigation of additional cut planes confirms these results [54].

    It is worth noting that all the included studies showed no effect ascribable to tDCS current strength and duration or number of sessions. This result is consistent with recent findings in electrical stimulation literature showing no significant difference in cortical excitability induced by these parameters, which might depend on the high degree of inter-subject variability in the neurophysiological response to tDCS [26]. However, the reported decreased excitability produced by long duration of tDCS [55] might be referred to phenomena of neuronal habituation.

    The current work has some limitations that need to be addressed. First of all, the small pool of included studies (N = 5) impede robust qualitative conclusions. Other issues concern the heterogeneity of included studies, that prevent a meta-analytic comparison, too.

    Additionally, the computation of the magnitude of the effects on food craving measures prevented robust conclusions, since most of the studies analyzed reported small or medium effects sizes.

    In addition, an analysis of potential confounders has been conducted to disentangle the issues of mixed results (Table 2).

    Table 2.  Possible confounding factors affecting the direction of results.
    Sample Type of design Blinding procedures Stimulation location Number of sessions and duration Outcome measures
    - Unbalanced distribution of female and male within studies - Crossover VS between subject VS within subject - Single blinded VS double blinded - Active stimulation: left vs right side; - Single session VS repeated sessions - Food craving outcomes: behavioral VS self-report measures
    - Return electrode: cephalic vs extracephalic - Duration: 20 min VS 40 min

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    In conclusion, the revised studies showed controversial results. Indeed, it is not possible to discern whether positive effects can be ascribable to expectancy effects or to tDCS effect itself. Therefore, future tDCS protocols, including expectancy effect control, should be adopted. As regards the target area, clarifying a possible lateralization in food intake regulation and comparing the anodal stimulation of both right and left DLPFC in the same study should be recommended. It is worth to note that tDCS has some technical limitations such as low spatial accuracy that prevent an optimal target precision (the current passes through the brain from anode to cathode and modulates neural activity simultaneously underneath anode and cathode, and it might be hard to associate the effects of tDCS to a specific brain area). In addition, long-term effect after tDCS use is not in depth investigated, and so not well established [56]. Lastly, the difference between active and sham session might be statistically but non clinically significant [57].

    tDCS technique is broadly safe, and does not cause permanent or severe damage or discomfort when used according to the safety guidelines. However, several risks have been reported, and they might represent therapeutic limitations. In fact, sensations such as tingling, itching, and burning sensations under the electrodes, mild headaches, fatigue, reported by individuals during and/or after the administration of tDCS [58] can decrease individuals' adherence to protocols, thus preventing therapeutic benefits. On the other hands, the increase of individuals' adherence might also represent the therapeutic goal of tDCS: for example, some studies reported an increased therapeutic adherence to treatments following tDCS, thus helping the self-management of individuals' health [59],[60].

    For the aforementioned reasons, in the field of obesity and overweight, it is possible to program personalized intervention to reduce weight and enhance subjects' quality of life with a more holistic intervention, as tDCS alone might not be sufficient to reach this aim. In fact, tDCS, combined with the use of psychoeducative intervention, diet and physical activity, might represents a potential to manage food craving in individuals with overweight and obesity.



    Conflict of interest



    The authors declare no conflict of interest.

    Author contributions



    GO devised the main research topic and planned the literature search. GO conceived the conceptual ideas and proof outline. GO, CC, AR and LM drafted the first version of the manuscript; GO, VC, and EM drafted the second version of the manuscript; GO, VC, EM, DM, CS contributed to the data interpretation; GO, VC, EM, CC, DM, AR, CS and AG revised the manuscript critically. All authors gave the final approval for the version to be published.

    [1] World Obesity Day [Internet]World Obesity Federation. [cited 2022 Jul 13]. Available from: https://www.worldobesity.org/what-we-do/world-obesity-day
    [2] Iasevoli M, Giantin V, Voci A, et al. (2012) Discussing end-of-life care issues with terminally ill patients and their relatives: comparisons among physicians, nurses and psychologists. Aging Clin Exp Res 24: 35-42.
    [3] Salari N, Hosseinian-Far A, Jalali R, et al. (2020) Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Globalization Health 16: 57. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-020-00589-w
    [4] Maharaj PE, Barclay N, Descartes C, et al. (2019) Intimate Partner Violence and Sleep: An Overview of Sleep Disturbances and Interventions. Handbook of Interpersonal Violence and Abuse Across the Lifespan: A project of the National Partnership to End Interpersonal Violence Across the Lifespan (NPEIV) [Internet]. Cham: Springer International Publishing p. 1-25. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-62122-7_132-1. [cited 2022 May 12]
    [5] Mattioli AV, Pinti M, Farinetti A, et al. (2020) Obesity risk during collective quarantine for the COVID-19 epidemic. Obes Med 20: 100263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obmed.2020.100263
    [6] Weingarten HP, Elston D (1990) The phenomenology of food cravings. Appetite 15: 231-246. https://doi.org/10.1016/0195-6663(90)90023-2
    [7] Pelchat ML, Johnson A, Chan R, et al. (2004) Images of desire: food-craving activation during fMRI. Neuroimage 23: 1486-1493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.023
    [8] Bénard M, Camilleri GM, Etilé F, et al. (2017) Association between Impulsivity and Weight Status in a General Population. Nutrients 9: E217. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9030217
    [9] Favieri F, Forte G, Casagrande M (2019) The Executive Functions in Overweight and Obesity: A Systematic Review of Neuropsychological Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Studies. Front Psychol . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02126
    [10] Basdevant A, Craplet C, Guy-Grand B (1993) Snacking patterns in obese French women. Appetite 21: 17-23. https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1993.1033
    [11] Narayanaswami V, Dwoskin LP (2017) Obesity: Current and potential pharmacotherapeutics and targets. Pharmacol Ther 170: 116-147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pharmthera.2016.10.015
    [12] Uher R, Murphy T, Brammer MJ, et al. (2004) Medial prefrontal cortex activity associated with symptom provocation in eating disorders. Am J Psychiatry 161: 1238-1246. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1238
    [13] Fabricatore AN, Wadden TA (2004) Psychological aspects of obesity. Clin Dermatol 22: 332-337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clindermatol.2004.01.006
    [14] Kim S, Lee D (2011) Prefrontal Cortex and Impulsive Decision Making. Biol Psychiatry 69: 1140-1146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.005
    [15] Szabo-Reed AN, Martin LE, Hu J, et al. (2020) Modeling interactions between brain function, diet adherence behaviors, and weight loss success. Obes Sci Pract 6: 282-292. https://doi.org/10.1002/osp4.403
    [16] Orrù G, Baroni M, Cesari V, et al. (2019) The effect of single and repeated tDCS sessions on motor symptoms in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review. Arch Ital Biol 157: 89-101. https://doi.org/10.12871/00039829201925
    [17] Orrù G, Cesari V, Baroni M, et al. (2020) Letter to the Editor: the application of transcranial direct current stimulation on phantom phenomena. Mediterr J Clin Psyc 8. Available from: https://cab.unime.it/journals/index.php/MJCP/article/view/2382
    [18] Orrù G, Cesari V, Conversano C, et al. (2021) The clinical application of transcranial direct current stimulation in patients with cerebellar ataxia: a systematic review. Int J Neurosci 131: 681-688. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207454.2020.1750399
    [19] Orrù G, Conversano C, Hitchcott PK, et al. (2020) Motor stroke recovery after tDCS: a systematic review. Rev Neurosci 31: 201-218. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2019-0047
    [20] Chen J, Qin J, He Q, et al. (2020) A Meta-Analysis of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Substance and Food Craving: What Effect Do Modulators Have?. Front Psychiatry 11: 598. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00598
    [21] Nitsche MA, Paulus W (2000) Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol 527: 633-639. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x
    [22] Nitsche MA, Fricke K, Henschke U, et al. (2003) Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J Physiol 553: 293-301. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2003.049916
    [23] Angius L, Santarnecchi E, Pascual-Leone A, et al. (2019) Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation over the Left Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Improves Inhibitory Control and Endurance Performance in Healthy Individuals. Neuroscience 419: 34-45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.08.052
    [24] Ota K, Shinya M, Kudo K (2019) Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Over Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Modulates Risk-Attitude in Motor Decision-Making. Front Hum Neurosci 13: 297. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00297
    [25] Friehs MA, Frings C, Hartwigsen G (2021) Effects of single-session transcranial direct current stimulation on reactive response inhibition. Neurosci Biobehav R 128: 749-65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.07.013
    [26] Lowe CJ, Vincent C, Hall PA (2017) Effects of Noninvasive Brain Stimulation on Food Cravings and Consumption: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychosom Med 79: 2-13. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000368
    [27] Mostafavi SA, Khaleghi A, Mohammadi MR, et al. (2020) Is transcranial direct current stimulation an effective modality in reducing food craving? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr Neurosci 23: 55-67. https://doi.org/10.1080/1028415X.2018.1470371
    [28] Orru G, Miniati M, Conversano C, et al. (2021) A machine learning analysis of psychopathological features of Eating Disorders: a retrospective study. Mediterr J Clin Psyc 9. Available from: https://cab.unime.it/journals/index.php/MJCP/article/view/2670
    [29] Meule A, Küppers C, Harms L, et al. (2018) Food cue-induced craving in individuals with bulimia nervosa and binge-eating disorder. PLOS ONE 13: e0204151. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0204151
    [30] Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, et al. (2009) Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ 339: b2535. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b2535
    [31] da Costa Santos CM, de Mattos Pimenta CA, Nobre MRC (2007) The PICO strategy for the research question construction and evidence search. Rev Lat Am Enfermagem 15: 508-511. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-11692007000300023
    [32] Mendeley - Reference Management Software [Internet]. [cited 2022 Jul 13]. Available from: https://www.mendeley.com/?interaction_required=true
    [33] Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D, et al. (1996) Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary?. Control Clin Trials 17: 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1016/0197-2456(95)00134-4
    [34] Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, et al. (2011) The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 343: d5928. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d5928
    [35] Grundeis F, Brand C, Kumar S, et al. (2017) Non-invasive Prefrontal/Frontal Brain Stimulation Is Not Effective in Modulating Food Reappraisal Abilities or Calorie Consumption in Obese Females. Front Neurosci 11: 334. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00334
    [36] Ray MK, Sylvester MD, Helton A, et al. (2019) The effect of expectation on transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to suppress food craving and eating in individuals with overweight and obesity. Appetite 136: 1-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.12.044
    [37] Marron EM, Viejo-Sobera R, Cuatrecasas G, et al. (2019) Prefronto-cerebellar neuromodulation affects appetite in obesity. Int J Obes (Lond) 43: 2119-2124. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41366-018-0278-8
    [38] Gluck ME, Alonso-Alonso M, Piaggi P, et al. (2015) Neuromodulation targeted to the prefrontal cortex induces changes in energy intake and weight loss in obesity. Obesity 23: 2149-2156. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.21313
    [39] Heinitz S, Reinhardt M, Piaggi P, et al. (2017) Neuromodulation directed at the prefrontal cortex of subjects with obesity reduces snack food intake and hunger in a randomized trial. Am J Clin Nutr 106: 1347-1357. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.117.158089
    [40] Lenhard W, Lenhard A Computation of Effect Sizes (2017).
    [41] Wilson SJ, Sayette MA, Fiez JA (2004) Prefrontal responses to drug cues: a neurocognitive analysis. Nat Neurosci 7: 211-214. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1200
    [42] Alonso-Alonso M, Pascual-Leone A (2007) The right brain hypothesis for obesity. JAMA 297: 1819-1822. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.297.16.1819
    [43] Legget KT, Wylie KP, Cornier MA, et al. (2020) Altered between-network connectivity in individuals prone to obesity. Physiol Behav 113242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113242
    [44] Son NT Le D, Pannacciulli N, Chen K, et al. (2007) Less activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the reanalysis of the response to a meal in obese than in lean women and its association with successful weight loss. Am J Clin Nutr 86: 573-579. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/86.3.573
    [45] Wager TD, Atlas LY (2015) The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nat Rev Neurosci 16: 403-418. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3976
    [46] Benedetti F, Carlino E, Pollo A (2011) How Placebos Change the Patient's Brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 339-354. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.81
    [47] Heekeren HR, Marrett S, Bandettini PA, et al. (2004) A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain. Nature 431: 859-862. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02966
    [48] Rolls ET, Critchley HD, Browning A, et al. (1998) The Neurophysiology of Taste and Olfaction in Primates, and Umami Flavora. Ann NY Acad Sci 855: 426-437. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb10602.x
    [49] Kumar S, Grundeis F, Brand C, et al. (2016) Differences in Insula and Pre-/Frontal Responses during Reappraisal of Food in Lean and Obese Humans. Front Hum Neurosci 10: 233. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00233
    [50] Berman BD, Horovitz SG, Hallett M (2013) Modulation of functionally localized right insular cortex activity using real-time fMRI-based neurofeedback. Front Hum Neurosci . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00638
    [51] Noori HR, Cosa Linan A, Spanagel R (2016) Largely overlapping neuronal substrates of reactivity to drug, gambling, food and sexual cues: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 26: 1419-1430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.06.013
    [52] Bryant M, Truesdale KP, Dye L (2006) Modest changes in dietary intake across the menstrual cycle: implications for food intake research. Br J Nutr 96: 888-894. https://doi.org/10.1017/BJN20061931
    [53] Accornero N, Capozza M, Pieroni L, et al. (2014) EEG mean frequency changes in healthy subjects during prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation. J Neurophysiol 112: 1367-1375. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00088.2014
    [54] Noetscher GM, Yanamadala J, Makarov SN, et al. (2014) Comparison of cephalic and extracephalic montages for transcranial direct current stimulation--a numerical study. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 61: 2488-2498. https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2014.2322774
    [55] Misonou H, Mohapatra DP, Park EW, et al. (2004) Regulation of ion channel localization and phosphorylation by neuronal activity. Nat Neurosci 7: 711-718. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1260
    [56] Prehn K, Flöel A (2015) Potentials and limits to enhance cognitive functions in healthy and pathological aging by tDCS. Front Cell Neurosci . https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2015.00355
    [57] Lefaucheur JP, Antal A, Ayache SS, et al. (2017) Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Clin Neurophysiol 128: 56-92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2016.10.087
    [58] Davis SE, Smith GA (2019) Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Use in Warfighting: Benefits, Risks, and Future Prospects. Front Hum Neurosci . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00114
    [59] Chang CC, Kao YC, Chao CY, et al. (2020) The Effects of Bi-Anodal tDCS Over the Prefrontal Cortex Regions With Extracephalic Reference Placement on Insight Levels and Cardio-Respiratory and Autonomic Functions in Schizophrenia Patients and Exploratory Biomarker Analyses for Treatment Response. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 24: 40-53. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyaa063
    [60] Saxena V, Pal A (2021) Role of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in the Management of Alzheimer's Disease: A Meta-analysis of Effects, Adherence and Adverse Effects. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci 19: 589-599. https://doi.org/10.9758/cpn.2021.19.4.589
  • neurosci-09-03-020-s001.pdf
  • This article has been cited by:

    1. Heverton Araújo de Oliveira Figueirêdo, Edson Silva-Filho, Antônio Felipe Cavalcante, Rodrigo Pegado, Anna Cecília Queiroz de Medeiros, Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation for Food Craving in Women Affected by Overweight and Obesity: A Randomized Controlled Trial, 2024, 30, 1022-6877, 197, 10.1159/000535720
    2. Olena Klimenko, Oscar Zamora-Contreras, Nubia Hernández - Flórez, Elizabet Ruiz-Zuluaga, Craving: explanatory models, associated factors, and therapeutic approaches: A systematic review, 2024, 18, 2145-2776, 10.25057/21452776.1627
    3. Vicente Javier Clemente-Suárez, Ana Isabel Beltrán-Velasco, Laura Redondo-Flórez, Alexandra Martín-Rodríguez, Rodrigo Yáñez-Sepúlveda, José Francisco Tornero-Aguilera, Neuro-Vulnerability in Energy Metabolism Regulation: A Comprehensive Narrative Review, 2023, 15, 2072-6643, 3106, 10.3390/nu15143106
    4. Sachin Kumar, Chandrakant Gawli, Chandragouda Patil, 2024, Chapter 16, 978-3-031-75685-6, 291, 10.1007/978-3-031-75686-3_16
  • Reader Comments
  • © 2022 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(2754) PDF downloads(258) Cited by(4)

Figures and Tables

Figures(1)  /  Tables(2)

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog