Research article Special Issues

The role of physical activity in the physiological activation of the scholastic pre-requirements

  • Received: 30 March 2024 Revised: 08 August 2024 Accepted: 14 August 2024 Published: 19 August 2024
  • Physical activity during the developmental age is an indispensable tool for the physical and mental growth of children. Thanks to physical activity, individuals have the opportunity to improve their physical efficiency and promote better health, establish relationships with the environment and with others, and develop cognitive processes. Therefore, the aim of this study is to investigate the relationship between physical activity and the development of scholastic prerequisites among kindergarten children. 52 children (aged 4–5) participated in either a classroom-based physical activity program (60′/3 days per week) or regular lessons. At the beginning and end of the intervention programs, a set of standardized motor evaluation tests and the Observational Questionnaire for the Early Identification of Learning Disabilities (IPDA) were administered. As a result, a meaningful Time x Group interaction for the IPDA Variable was observed. The aforementioned development denotes a noteworthy advancement within the treatment group (p < 0.001). Conversely, no substantial modification was noted in the control group. The findings derived from this study provide a foundational support to the concept that physical activity integrated into classroom settings is an effective strategy to improve both scholastic prerequisites and academic performance.

    Citation: Francesca Latino, Francesco Tafuri. The role of physical activity in the physiological activation of the scholastic pre-requirements[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(3): 244-259. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024016

    Related Papers:

    [1] Blai Ferrer-Uris, Maria Angeles Ramos, Albert Busquets, Rosa Angulo-Barroso . Can exercise shape your brain? A review of aerobic exercise effects on cognitive function and neuro-physiological underpinning mechanisms. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(2): 150-174. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022009
    [2] Mark R. Zielinski, James T. McKenna, Robert W. McCarley . Functions and Mechanisms of Sleep. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(1): 67-104. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.1.67
    [3] Laura Serra, Sara Raimondi, Carlotta di Domenico, Silvia Maffei, Anna Lardone, Marianna Liparoti, Pierpaolo Sorrentino, Carlo Caltagirone, Laura Petrosini, Laura Mandolesi . The beneficial effects of physical exercise on visuospatial working memory in preadolescent children. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(4): 496-509. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021026
    [4] Nisha Shantakumari, Musaab Ahmed . Whole body vibration therapy and cognitive functions: a systematic review. AIMS Neuroscience, 2023, 10(2): 130-143. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2023010
    [5] Ida Ayu Sri Wijayanti, I Made Oka Adnyana, I Putu Eka Widyadharma, I Gede Eka Wiratnaya, Tjokorda Gde Bagus Mahadewa, I Nyoman Mantik Astawa . Neuroinflammation mechanism underlying neuropathic pain: the role of mesenchymal stem cell in neuroglia. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(3): 226-243. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024015
    [6] Sairam Ramesh, Arosh S. Perera Molligoda Arachchige . Depletion of dopamine in Parkinson's disease and relevant therapeutic options: A review of the literature. AIMS Neuroscience, 2023, 10(3): 200-231. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2023017
    [7] Evgenia Lazarou, Themis P. Exarchos . Predicting stress levels using physiological data: Real-time stress prediction models utilizing wearable devices. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(2): 76-102. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024006
    [8] Antonio G. Lentoor, Tiro B. Motsamai, Thandokuhle Nxiweni, Bongumusa Mdletshe, Siyasanga Mdingi . Protocol for a systematic review of the effects of gardening physical activity on neuroplasticity and cognitive function. AIMS Neuroscience, 2023, 10(2): 118-129. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2023009
    [9] Myungjin Jung, Han Soo Kim, Paul D Loprinzi, Minsoo Kang . Serial-multiple mediation of enjoyment and intention on the relationship between creativity and physical activity. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(1): 161-180. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021008
    [10] Damien Moore, Paul D Loprinzi . The association of self-reported physical activity on human sensory long-term potentiation. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(3): 435-447. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021023
  • Physical activity during the developmental age is an indispensable tool for the physical and mental growth of children. Thanks to physical activity, individuals have the opportunity to improve their physical efficiency and promote better health, establish relationships with the environment and with others, and develop cognitive processes. Therefore, the aim of this study is to investigate the relationship between physical activity and the development of scholastic prerequisites among kindergarten children. 52 children (aged 4–5) participated in either a classroom-based physical activity program (60′/3 days per week) or regular lessons. At the beginning and end of the intervention programs, a set of standardized motor evaluation tests and the Observational Questionnaire for the Early Identification of Learning Disabilities (IPDA) were administered. As a result, a meaningful Time x Group interaction for the IPDA Variable was observed. The aforementioned development denotes a noteworthy advancement within the treatment group (p < 0.001). Conversely, no substantial modification was noted in the control group. The findings derived from this study provide a foundational support to the concept that physical activity integrated into classroom settings is an effective strategy to improve both scholastic prerequisites and academic performance.



    In recent years, the scientific community has recognized the body and movement not only as the same dignity as other intellectual forms, but a dimension thanks to which they are recognized as accelerators of human learning processes [1]. Starting from these assumptions, a growing scientific interest has developed in the effects of physical activity on cognitive functioning and brain health [2]. This interest has been largely driven by the contribution of neuroscientific studies that have linked physical activity to cognitive function, as well as to brain structure and function [3]. The first evidence of the direct effects of exercise on the brain was obtained from research conducted on animals [4]. Subsequently, a growing body of data from laboratory studies began to provide the first evidence about the potential cellular and molecular mechanisms through which physical activity was able to influence brain structure and function, with concomitant benefits on cognitive processes [5].

    Cognitive processes are those functions through which the person can direct attention, acquire and process information from the surrounding world, recall the most important information and neglect the less important ones, and make decisions in relation to the needs [6][11]. Over the last few decades, numerous studies have shown that physical activity has a clear influence on cognitive functions, especially on processes which involve learning skills and memory [12]. It is able to improve the fundamental aspects that underlie school skills, such as concentration, working memory, classroom behavior, self-esteem, and a sense of perceived self-efficacy [13]. In fact, physical activity can positively regulate brain processes related to learning mechanisms, which induce significant functional and structural changes at the level of the Central Nervous System (CNS).

    The relationship between motor development and cognitive performance was probably first established in children [14]; however, evidence of the benefits of exercise on human cognition has been more fully developed in research with older adults. Many of these experiments have clearly shown that regular physical activity alters specific brain structures and functions, which can lead to an improvement in cognitive performance [15]. Already in early academic papers, researchers concluded that children with better physical fitness showed a greater allocation of attentional resources to working memory [16]. In addition, Sibley and Etnier [17], who more than any other have laid the foundations of research in this field, have confirmed the existence of a small but significant connection between physical activity and cognitive performance in school-age children (4 years), and suggested that physical activity may be related to positive cognitive outcomes during development.

    Early scientific studies on the relationship between physical activity and cognitive functions suggested that even short-term exercise was able to improve several aspects of cognitive function immediately after the completion of the activity, regardless of the level of physical efficiency [18]. However, until recently, the relationship between strenuous exercise and cognitive function was not fully established since the literature on the subject seemed to provide somewhat contradictory results. In fact, while several studies indicated that short sessions of physical activity could induce benefits on cognitive functioning [19], others found no beneficial effect [20].

    In recent years, research in this field has considerably evolved through studies born from the need to better understand the multifaceted relationship between physical activity and cognitive health. Advances in neuroscience and neuroimaging techniques have allowed researchers to better understand the effects of physical activity on cognitive functions by investigating their physiological activation and molecular mechanisms [5]. The research conducted in the different areas of interest of physical activity has helped to define this line of investigation and to identify some underlying preliminary mechanisms. This has led to a deeper understanding of the components that are related to cognitive function and those that may be amenable to intervention [21]. In more recent times, it has been shown that the effect of physical activity on cognitive performance depends on both the intensity and duration of exercise [22], as well as on the intrinsic aspects that characterize the proposed activity (i.e., the cognitive effort required by exercise) [23]. The most recent scientific evidence showed how the effects of physical activity on cognitive abilities could lead to favorable results in terms of improvements of both low-level cognitive functions related to simple reactions and of higher-level functions such as memory, processing speed, attention, and executive function [24][26]. Specifically, many studies argue that, through physical activity, it is possible to contribute to improving the cognitive and instrumental prerequisites necessary to face the first school learning and develop metacognitive skills [27],[28] more easily. This happens because learning is formed through a process of internalization of the activities carried out at the motor and bodily levels. Training on pre-requirements before even starting primary school allows for a better evolution of learning. During the period of kindergarten, the child begins to develop different skills and competences. Although they are not yet scholastically learning, they will be indispensable in Primary School, as they form the basis on which skills will be developed for all future learning. These basic skills are called school pre-requirements and are divided into four macro-categories, within which specific skills can be identified. They include the following: communication and language skills, visual-motor skills, attentional skills, and executive functions (inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility, planning, and problem solving). According to an approach of the early identification of learning difficulties and the timely intervention of enhancements, it is desirable to identify valid and effective strategies to develop these skills [29][31].

    The school environment is an important arena for creating good life habits, initiating changes, and developing cognitive performance. In Italy, there is concern that physical education takes valuable time away from academic work, so much so that it is often the idea that the time allocated for physical education will be drastically reduced by changes in the school curriculum. However, allocating time for physical education does not seem to compromise the academic results. More research is needed to address the benefits of school-based physical activity and to support and motivate relevant groups and policy makers to create policies and environments that support increased physical activity in educational settings [32].

    Therefore, the aim of the present study is to explore the relationship between physical activity and the development of school pre-requirements, hypothesizing that the physiological activation of the body and its movement can be an effective means of mediation between the environment and the development of cognitive functions.

    This study was a randomized controlled trial that was structured to investigate the mediating role function of a school-based physical activity (PA) program on cognitive performance and scholastic prerequisites among school-age children. The research was executed in a kindergarten, where the participants engaged in a PA regimen implemented in a curricular class. Hence, the program of intervention was carried out for a whole school year and encompassed a total of 82 sessions that involved moderate to intense aerobic workouts (MVPA) for the experimental group and standard classes for the control group. The intervention entailed three 60-minute physical activity sessions per week conducted during regular school hours. The assessments were conducted prior to and upon completion of the intervention schemes.

    As for the randomization protocol, the simple randomization method through the random number table was used. An electronic tool to generate numerical sequences was used for this purpose. Subsequently, it was established as an allocation rule that subjects that corresponded to “even” digits would fall into the experimental group (EG), while all those that corresponded to “odd” digits would fall into the control group (CG). The research team was kept unaware of the group assignments.

    52 participants, ranging in age from 4 to 5 years (mean age = 4.70, SD = ± 0.46), were selected from a kindergarten situated in the southern region of Italy.

    The inclusion criteria was comprised of individuals who possessed the capability to engage in a moderate-to-vigorous intensity aerobic exercise session, those who were enrolled as students in the institution, and those who demonstrated the ability to refrain from any physical activities beyond the confines of the study protocol on the testing days. Children who could not refrain from exercising outside the study guidelines were omitted from the research. It should be noted that participation in the research was entirely voluntary. Parents and their children were educated on the attributes of the research. The researchers guaranteed the confidentiality of the subjects, and the procedure adhered to the guidelines outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all the parents. The research was carried out between 01 October 2023 and 30 May 2023.

    Prior to the inaugural school-based physical activity session, a comprehensive briefing was conducted to elucidate the contents of the program, while also confirming the individual motivation of each child. A week prior to the commencement of the intervention, the participants were escorted to the school gymnasium for the purpose of conducting standardized motor assessment tests and a cognitive evaluation. The capability of the participants to engage in a moderate-to-vigorous intensity aerobic exercise session was assessed using the OMNI scale. Moreover, each training session intensity was monitored through an OMNI scale to respect exertion in the MVPA range of a 5 < RPE < 8 and to prevent any differences between training sessions [33].

    The participants of the research underwent individual testing, in which each assessment was conducted sequentially, at a consistent time of day, and under comparable conditions. The evaluations were conducted both prior to and following the intervention, which allowed for the examination of the program's effects. The design, supervision, and implementation of all assessments and physical activities were overseen by a pair of physical education instructors, both pre and post the physical activity interventions.

    After the administration of the pre-tests, the children of the experimental group participated in an intervention to strengthen the school prerequisites through a recreational-motor path, which lasted 7 months. On the other hand, the children in the control group followed the normal preschool educational program proposed by the curricular teachers, without specific motor content, which mostly included free play either in the classroom or in the garden and preschool reading-writing activities with the use of operational notebooks.

    The physical activity intervention was comprised of a warm-up period that lasted 10 minutes, followed by a core session that lasted 40 minutes, and concluded with a cool-down period of 10 minutes. Within the core session, there was a sequence of activities aimed at enhancing engagement, motivation, and pleasure. Evidently, the fitness regimen was formulated to be pleasurable and attractive, with the aim of fostering a comprehensive recognition of one's own capabilities. These components could be succinctly outlined as follows:

    • Individual exercises;

    • Cooperative activities;

    • Exercises and techniques designed to improve lateralization, control, as well as overall and segmental coordination;

    • Coordination games and exploratory tasks to acquire proficiency in spatial awareness;

    • Utilization of the body parts in activities centered on enhancing focus and mindfulness;

    • Physical exercises and leisurely pursuits that involve pairs or teams, often structured to foster unity, mutual support within the group, and confidence in others;

    • Utilization of the body to convey, express, and portray both real and imaginary scenarios, personal experiences, emotions, and sentiments; and

    • Engagement in rhythmic tasks and games accompanied by music.

    The assessment examined six subtests that were included in the Motorfit battery [34]. These include the following: 1) Motorfit Locomotor ((Performing forward jumps on one foot (SAP1), executing lateral galloping (GL), and completing forward hopping steps on one foot (SAP2)); and 2) Motorfit Object ((Throwing a ball with one hand (LP), catching a ball with hands (RP), and striking a ball with a tennis racket (CP)).

    They were selected based on their straightforward and efficient execution, with minimal equipment being necessary.

    The Observational Questionnaire for the Early Identification of Learning Disabilities (IPDA) [35] is an observational questionnaire that must be completed by the teacher and is aimed at children in the last year of kindergarten. It allows the early identification of learning difficulties and allows a path that consists of three phases: i) screening; ii) more precise assessment of the state of development of specific skills (administration of IPDA batteries); and iii) targeted rehabilitation intervention for deficit areas.

    It consists of 43 items divided into two sections: 1) General skills (items 1–9 behavioral aspects, items 10–11 motor skills, items 12–14 language comprehension, items 15–19 oral expression, items 20–23 metacognition, and items 24–33 other cognitive skills); and 2) Specific skills (items 34–40 pre-literacy, and items 41–43 pre-mathematics). The teacher rates each item following a 4-point Likert scale: 1. Not at all/never; 2. A little/sometimes; 3. Somewhat/most of the time; and 4. A lot/always.

    The total score is obtained by summing the scores of the individual items. The total score makes it possible to identify the presence or absence of difficulties in the school prerequisites: high-risk band score ≤ 107; medium-high risk range score between 108 and 118; medium-low risk range score between 119 and 135; and low risk group score ≥ 136.

    Statistical analyses were conducted utilizing IBM SPSS, version 25.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). The data were depicted as group mean (M) values and standard deviations (SD) and were assessed for assumptions of normality, specifically through the Shapiro-Wilk test, and a homogeneity of variances, which involved the Levene test, within the data distributions. An independent sample t-test was executed to assess the group distinctions at baseline, while a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) (group (experimental/control) × time (pre/post-intervention)), with repeated measures on the time dimension, was carried out to explore the impact of the intervention on all dependent variables. When the interactions of “Group x Time” attained a statistical significance, subsequent pairwise comparisons were carried out using group-specific post hoc tests, specifically paired t-tests. The extent of the significant “Time x Group” interaction was quantified using partial eta squared (η2p) and was assessed based on the predetermined thresholds: small (η2p < 0.06), medium (0.06 ≤ η2p < 0.14), and large (η2p ≥ 0.14). The effect sizes for the comparisons were evaluated through Cohen's d, with values falling into the following categories: small (0.20 ≤ d < 0.50), moderate (0.50 ≤ d < 0.79), and large (d ≥ 0.80), according to Cohen [36]. The statistical significance level was established at p < 0.05.

    Both groups of participants exhibited no significant variance at the commencement in terms of age, anthropometric traits, and psychological assessments (p > 0.05) (Table 1). The outcomes pre- and post-intervention for all variables of interest are delineated in Table 2.

    Table 1.  Characteristic of participants.
    Variable EG (n = 26)
    Mean ± SD
    CG (n = 26)
    Mean ± SD
    Age (y)  4.61 ± 0.49 4.62 ± 0.73
    Height (cm) 114.50 ± 1.58 113.84 ± 1.37
    Weight (kg) 16.88 ± 3.00 16.90 ± 3.66
    Body mass index (kg.m− 2 - percentile) 23.20 ± 34.59 24.63 ± 37.28
    Sex, n (%)
    Male 17 (65.38) 9 (34.62)
    Female 14 (53.85) 12 (46.15)

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Table 2.  Changes after classroom-based PA intervention program.
    Experimental Group (n = 26)
    Control Group (n = 26)
    Baseline Post-test Δ Baseline Post-test Δ
    Motorfit Locomotor 8.73 (1.15) 9.65 (1.12) †* 0.92 (0.89) 8.68 (1.93) 8.60 (1.87) -0.08 (0.57)
    Motorfit Object 8.38 (0.89) 9.61 (1.06) †* 1.23 (0.90) 9.16 (1.31) 8.76 (1.36) -0.40 (0.64)
    IPDA Questionnaire 115.57 (1.55) 125.42 (1.55) †* 9.84 (3.10) 115.68 (1.49) 114.64 (2.62) -1.04 (2.20)

    Note: values are presented as mean (± SD); Δ: pre- to post-training changes; †Significant ‘Group x Time' interaction: significant effect of the intervention (p < 0.001). *Significantly different from pre-test (p < 0.001).

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Statistical analysis revealed a significant “Time x Group” interaction for Motorfit Locomotor (F1,50 = 23.47, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.31, large effect size) and Motorfit Object (F1,50 = 56.27, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.53 large effect size). A post-hoc analysis revealed a positive for Motorfit Locomotor (t = 5.28, p < 0.001, d = 1.03, large effect size) and Motorfit Object (t = 6.91, p < 0.001, d = 1.35, large effect size) in the intervention group. No significant changes were found for the control group (p > 0.05).

    The statistical analysis revealed a significant “Time x Group” interaction for IPDA (F1,50 = 214.90, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.81, large effect size). A post-hoc analysis revealed a positive decrease in IPDA (t = 16.15, p < 0.001, d = 3.16, large effect size) in the intervention group. Additionally, the results indicated that there was a significant main effect of the gender factor (F1,50 (1.22), p < 0.001, η2 = 0.84], which was higher in the male participants. No significant changes were found for the control group (p > 0.05).

    The objective of this study was to explore the correlation between physical activity intervention and scholastic prerequisites among kindergarten children, based on the assumption that the physiological activation due to physical activity plays a crucial role as a mediating factor. The findings from this research indicated that physical activity integrated in the scholastic curriculum proved to be successful in enhancing academic achievement. Conversely, traditional classroom sessions demonstrated a lower efficacy in attaining outcomes that aligned with the objectives outlined in this investigation. In fact, the main significant result that could be observed concerned the improvement in scholastic prerequisites as a consequence of physical activity. This outcome is likely attributed to the phenomenon where children, within the realm of physical activity, can encounter diverse physiological mechanisms that stem from acquiring a new skill. These processes are further bolstered by the intrinsic motivation to effectively participate in the activity on which they were focusing their time and energy [37][40]. Moreover, research in the field of neuroscience highlighted a profound interdependence between physical activity and cognitive processes [41][43]. According to a conceptualization model proposed by many authors [44][48], this interdependence is primarily related to changes that occur in brain function and structure as a consequence of physical activity.

    Investigations into the influence of physical activity on cognitive functioning have proposed several mechanisms that could explain this relationship [49][53]. Numerous studies published in recent years on this topic have mainly concluded that physical activity is able to determine a series of physiological and structural modifications and adaptations that allow brain cells to create new connections in different cortical areas. In fact, physical activitiy is able to trigger a cascade of neurochemical growth factors capable of changing the entire brain structure. This reflects the brain's ability to adapt to the various cognitive challenges it faces [54],[55].

    Stillman, Cohen, Lehman, and Erikson [56] proposed a conceptual model that identified multiple ways in which changes in brain structure, function, and behaviors, as well as in social-emotional functions (mood, motivation, or sleep), could mediate improvements in cognitive performance following physical activity practice. This model was the only one to highlight the idea that the mechanisms which underlie improvements in cognitive performance could be conceptualized on multiple levels: Level 1. Molecular and cellular mechanisms that occur as a result of physical activity; Level 2. Structural and functional modifications and adaptations of the brain; and Level 3. Behavioral and socio-emotional mechanisms. In relation to Level 1, and in particular to molecular mechanisms, exercise seems to exert its positive effects on cognitive functions by modulating the cascades of key growth factors responsible for synaptic plasticity. The concept of plasticity is fundamental to understand how exercise can optimize brain function by promoting the quality of learning [57]. Neuroplasticity is a constant and continuous process capable of modifying existing neuronal networks by mediating the structural and functional adaptations of synapses in response to changes in behavior [58]; additionally, this is in relation to the exercise and repeatability of similar networks. Neuroplasticity is a sort of structural and functional disposition of our nervous system to change as a result of stimuli from the environment: the richer and more varied the stresses, the more our brain will be able to adapt and modulate itself in response to them [59]. In fact, the human brain is not made up of fixed and immutable neural circuits; however, the cerebral synaptic network and the structures connected to it are capable of actively reorganizing themselves thanks to practice and experience [60] by reprogramming their neuronal networks. One of the molecules believed to be involved in these processes is the Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF), whose levels are increased following physical activity and closely correlate with the improvement in learning and memory described in rodents that underwent physical training [61]. BDNF is a polypeptide known as one of the major modulators of brain plasticity [62]. By increasing BDNF synthesis, physical activity would induce the creation of an ideal neurobiological environment that would become a significantly predisposed ground to accommodate the changes and adaptations induced by learning processes [63]. The cognitive aspects that would seem to benefit most from this increase in the BDNF protein are memory and executive functions, as well as an improved performance related to attention, inhibitory control, work speed, and visual learning [64]. Two other molecules potentially involved in the effects of physical activity on cognitive functions are Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor (VEGF) and Insulin-Like Growth Factor 1 (IGF1), which are responsible for activating neoangiogenesis processes in the brain [65]. VEGF, which is a subfamily of growth factors involved in vasculogenesis and angiogenesis, promotes the proliferation of neuronal precursors and provides a vascular heritage that is useful for the growth of neurons. IGF-1, which is a protein hormone with a molecular structure similar to insulin, is important for the development and maintenance of the nervous system. Physical activity can increase gene expression and protein levels of BDNF, IGF-1, and VEGF in different regions of the brain, particularly in the hippocampus [66].

    As far as cellular mechanisms are concerned, they are associated with phenomena related to neurogenesis and angiogenesis as neural mechanisms that mediate the beneficial cognitive effects of physical activity [67]. They are the result of increased oxygenation and irrigation of tissues, as well as an increased metabolic activity in response to exercise [68]. Angiogenesis, namely the development of new blood vessels, and neurogenesis (i.e., the development of new neurons), are complex cellular changes that result from the increased production of growth factors and upregulated molecular cascades [69]. The process of neurogenesis mainly occurs in the hippocampus (especially in the dentate gyrus), which is an area specialized in spatial learning and the consolidation of short- and long-term memory. Neurogenesis phenomena, starting from neonatal cells, could help to explain the effects of physical activity on learning and memory functions [70].

    Most of the studies that have examined the mechanisms which underlie the level 2 analysis have focused on the effects of physical activity in relation to brain structure, particularly in regard to gray matter volume and changes in the white matter microstructure, especially in the temporal and prefrontal region. These modifications would partly explain the cognitive improvements after exercise [71],[72].

    Chaddock et al. [73] systematically demonstrated the effects of physical activity on brain structure and function and the existence of a positive relationship between aerobic fitness, brain volume, and cognitive abilities. In fact, the authors argue that higher levels of aerobic fitness were related to higher academic achievement, improved cognitive abilities, larger brain structures, and higher brain function in school-age children (9–10 years). Through the use of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), the authors found that fitter children have a high ability to activate the frontal and parietal brain regions important for monitoring, maintaining, and strategizing higher-level cognitive control skills, which are skills that are important to achieve an improved learning performance. Additionally, they showed that fitter children had larger bilateral hippocampal volumes and higher performances on memory tasks compared to less fit children. Interestingly, these results differed from the results of the studies used in the meta-analysis by Sibley and Etnier [17], in which the authors suggested that physical activity was not related to memory skills in children aged 4 to 18 years [74].

    Hillman et al. [75] showed that children that participated in at least 70 minutes per day of intense physical activity for nine months, directed toward increasing aerobic capacity, significantly improved the brain function and behavioral indices of executive control. The authors suggested that fitness-related benefits appeared to follow an observed dose-response relationship between the participation rate, brain function, and executive control, and showed that brain and behavioral changes were a function of the degree of participation in the physical activity program. Hillman and colleagues found that fitter children outperformed less fit children in Eriksen's Flanker test (a conflict resolution test). In addition to the behavioral measures, the authors studied event-related brain potentials (ERPs) derived from electroencephalographic (EEG) activity through 64 electrodes that collected P3 amplitude and latency measurements during cognitive tasks. They showed that, compared to controls, children that participated in the physical activity program showed more pronounced changes in neural indices of attention (P3 amplitude), processing speed (P3 latency), and an improved performance during executive control tasks, which reflected the greater allocation of attentional resources and a greater cognitive processing speed.

    The data obtained in this investigation also suggested that statistically notable gender disparities were observed between boys and girls. Indeed, scores acquired by boys were elevated; these findings corroborate certain research [76][80], which suggested that boys universally achieved higher scores. These global disparities concerning gender could be attributed, in part, to the elevated scores acquired by girls in manual dexterity [81] and in balance [82], which were possibly generated by the cultivation of practices of stereotypical activities, and various sports activities, among boys and girls. Young elaborated on the distinctions in human throwing and hitting conduct from an evolutionary standpoint. Early humans sustained themselves by throwing rocks and wielding clubs. Women allocated more resources towards reproduction, while men were more inclined towards being hunters and fighters. These sorts of trends were passed down through natural selection [83]. A prior examination theorized that throwing is more likely an inherent ability whose progression is biologically predetermined and rather challenging to be impacted by nurture; the same might hold true for striking [84]. Moreover, societal aspects and behavioral tendencies might contribute to gender disparities in skillfulness in object manipulation.

    This study offered support for the positive correlation between physical activity and academic performance; however, certain limitations within the research necessitate further examination. In particular, the study was constrained by the fact that it only included students from a single educational institution, which limited the generalizability of the results to students from diverse school settings or varying demographic backgrounds. Another limitation concerned the fact that mid-term assessments were not conducted over 7 months. Furthermore, the small sample size of 52 participants due to the difficulties in engaging participants served as another notable limitation. Lastly, the study also failed to assess the socio-emotional variables associated with physical activity and the school environment, which presented another area of limitation. The scientific evidence described above implies a series of physiological, molecular, cellular, and cerebral processes involved in the relationship between physical activity and cognition; however, at a more in-depth level of analysis, it is likely that it also exerts its positive influence in relation to human and socio-emotional behavior [53]. Moreover, the study fails to investigate a broad spectrum of age groups and was limited to data gathered within a specific timeframe. Hence, it is advisable that future studies should examine comparable variables across a more diverse and extensive sample. Nonetheless, the results obtained could offer valuable insights for upcoming research endeavors. Thus, the effectiveness of this study was bolstered by a strategic approach that enhances not only physical fitness and academic prerequisites, but also scholastic performance.

    The findings derived from the current study enable us to assert with increased certainty that engaging in physical activity leads to significant effects that facilitate enhancements in cognitive function, thereby offering various avenues to enhance the educational methods of school-aged individuals. Moreover, facilitating regular physical activity among children during the academic day has the potential to enhance academic performance and simultaneously foster a balanced mind-body growth. Hence, given the acknowledged positive correlation between physical activity and academic requirements, physical activity represents a beneficial avenue to encourage a dynamic lifestyle among school-aged children. Since the educational institution serves as the primary environment where children spend most of their time, it is imperative for the school setting to facilitate opportunities to meet the recommended levels of physical activity through more robust initiatives to extend the duration dedicated to physical exercise.



    Funding



    No sources of funding were used to assist in the preparation of this manuscript.

    Conflicts of interest



    The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

    Authors' contribution



    Author 1 designed the study, conducted the research, carried out the statistical analysis, interpreted the data, wrote, and revised the manuscript. Author 2 supervised the intervention program, collected data, was involved in the interpretation of data, and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed intellectually to the manuscript, and all authors have read the manuscript and approved the submission.

    Use of AI tools declaration



    The authors declare they have not used Artificial Intelligence (AI) tools in the creation of this article.

    [1] Sibilio M (2002) Il corpo intelligente. Napoli: Simone.
    [2] Ploughman M (2008) Exercise is brain food: the effects of physical activity on cognitive function. Dev Neurorehabil 11: 236-240. https://doi.org/10.1080/17518420801997007
    [3] Gregory SM, Parker B, Thompson PD (2012) Physical activity, cognitive function, and brain health: what is the role of exercise training in the prevention of dementia?. Brain Sci 2: 684-708. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci2040684
    [4] Erickson KI, Hillman CH, Kramer AF (2015) Physical activity, brain, and cognition. Curr Opin Behav Sci 4: 27-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.005
    [5] Bidzan-Bluma I, Lipowska M (2018) Physical Activity and Cognitive Functioning of Children: A Systematic Review. Int J Env Res Public He 15: 800. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15040800
    [6] Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D, et al. (2016) Physical Activity, Fitness, Cognitive Function, and Academic Achievement in Children: A Systematic Review. Med Sci Sport Exer 48: 1197-1222. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0000000000000901
    [7] Latino F, Saraiello E, Tafuri F (2023) Outdoor Physical Activity: A Training Method for Learning in an Experiential and Innovative Way. J Phys Educ Sport (JPES) 23: 1852-1860.
    [8] Latino F, Tafuri F (2024a) Physical Activity and Cognitive Functioning. Medicina 60: 216. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60020216
    [9] Tafuri F, Latino F (2024b) School Medical Service: Strategies to Promote Psycho-Physiological Well-Being. Pediatric Rep 16: 214-231. https://doi.org/10.3390/pediatric16010019
    [10] Latino F, Tafuri F (2023) Physical Activity and Academic Performance in School-Age Children: A Systematic Review. Sustainability 15: 6616. https://doi.org/10.3390/su15086616
    [11] Latino F, Tafuri F, Saraiello E, et al. (2023) Classroom-based Physical Activity as a Means to Improve Self-efficacy and Academic Achievement among Normal-weight and Overweight Youth. Nutrients 15: 2061. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15092061
    [12] Cassilhas RC, Tufik S, de Mello MT (2016) Physical exercise, neuroplasticity, spatial learning and memory. Cell Mol Life Sci 73: 975-983. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00018-015-2102-0
    [13] Esteban-Cornejo I, Tejero-Gonzalez CM, Sallis JF, et al. (2015) Physical activity and cognition in adolescents: A systematic review. J Sci Med Sport 18: 534-539. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2014.07.007
    [14] Clarke HH (1958) Physical fitness benefits: A summary of research. Education 78: 460-466.
    [15] Calcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, et al. (2006) Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A-Biol 61: 1166-1170. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/61.11.1166
    [16] Castelli DM, Hillman CH, Buck SM, et al. (2007) Physical Fitness and Academic Achievement in Third- and Fifth-Grade Students. J Sport Exercise Psy 29: 239-252. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.29.2.239
    [17] Sibley BA, Etnier JL (2003) The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: a meta-analysis. Pediatr Exerc Sci 15: 243-56. https://doi.org/10.1123/pes.15.3.243
    [18] Tomporowski PD (2003) Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol 112: 297-324. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00134-8
    [19] Hancock S, McNaughton L (1986) Effects of fatigue on ability to process visual information by experienced orienteers. Percept Motor Skill 62: 491-498. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1986.62.2.491
    [20] Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, McDowell JE, et al. (2011) Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol 30: 91-98. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021766
    [21] Khan NA, Hillman CH (2014) The relation of childhood physical activity and aerobic fitness to brain function and cognition: a review. Pediatr Exerc Sci 26: 138-146. https://doi.org/10.1123/pes.2013-0125
    [22] Kamijo K, Nishihira Y, Higashiura T, et al. (2007) The interactive effect of exercise intensity and task difficulty on human cognitive processing. Int J Psychophysiol 65: 114-121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.04.001
    [23] Pesce C (2012) Shifting the focus from quantitative to qualitative exercise characteristics in exercise and cognition research. J Sport Exercise Psy 34: 766-786. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.34.6.766
    [24] Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, et al. (2011) Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. P Natl Acad Sci USA 108: 3017-3022. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1015950108
    [25] Latino F, Cataldi S, Carvutto R, et al. (2021) The importance of lipidomic approach for mapping and exploring the molecular networks underlying physical exercise: A systematic review. Int J Mol Sci 22: 8734. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms22168734
    [26] Latino F, Cataldi S, Fischetti F (2021) Effects of an 8-week yoga-based physical exercise intervention on teachers' burnout. Sustainability 13: 2104. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13042104
    [27] Mondoni M, Salvetti C (2016) La nuova didattica del movimento. Laboratori di giocosport e giochi inclusivi. Città di Castello: Mondadori Università.
    [28] Perotta F, Corona F, Cozzarelli C (2011) The efficacy of the project motorfit: Educational Actions through physical activity in schools. Sport Sci 4: 34-39.
    [29] Gajos A, Kujawski S, Gajos M, et al. (2014) Effect of physical activity on cognitive functions in elderly. J Health Sci 4: 91-100.
    [30] Morsanuto S, Peluso Cassese F, Tafuri F, et al. (2023) Outdoor Education, Integrated Soccer Activities, and Learning in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Project Aimed at Achieving the Sustainable Development Goals of the 2030 Agenda. Sustainability 15: 13456. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151813456
    [31] La Torre ME, Monda A, Messina A, et al. (2023) The potential role of nutrition in Overtraining syndrome. Nutrients 15: 4916. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15234916
    [32] Bidzan-Bluma I, Lipowska M (2018) Physical activity and cognitive functioning of children: a systematic review. Int J Env Res Public He 15: 800. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15040800
    [33] Utter AC, Robertson RJ, Nieman DC, et al. (2002) Children's OMNI Scale of Perceived Exertion: walking/running evaluation. Med Sci Sports Exerc 34: 139-144. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005768-200201000-00021
    [34] Perotta F, Corona F, Cozzarelli C (2011) The efficacy of the project motorfit: Educational Actions through physical activity in schools. Sport Sci 4: 34-39.
    [35] Terreni A, Tretti ML, Corcella PR, et al. (2011) IPDA. Questionario Osservativo per l'Identificazione Precoce delle Difficoltà di Apprendimento. Con CD-ROM (3a ed.). Trento: Erickson.
    [36] Cohen J (1992) A Power Primer. Psychol Bull 112: 155-159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
    [37] Latino F, Tafuri F, Saraiello E, et al. (2023) Classroom-based Physical Activity as a Means to Improve Self-efficacy and Academic Achievement among Normal-weight and Overweight Youth. Nutrients 15: 2061. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15092061
    [38] Farì G, Latino F, Tafuri F, et al. (2023) Shoulder Pain Biomechanics, Rehabilitation and Prevention in Wheelchair Basketball Players: A Narrative Review. Biomechanics 3: 362-376. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomechanics3030030
    [39] Latino F, Tafuri F (2024b) Wearable Sensors and the Evaluation of Physiological Performance in Elite Field Hockey Players. Sports 12: 124. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports12050124
    [40] Aidar FJ, Cataldi S, Badicu G, et al. (2022) Paralympic Powerlifting as a Sustainable Way to Improve Strength in Athletes with Spinal Cord Injury and Other Disabilities. Sustainability 14: 2017. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14042017
    [41] Latino F, Fischetti F, Cataldi S, et al. (2021) The Impact of an 8-Weeks At-Home Physical Activity Plan on Academic Achievement at the Time of COVID-19 Lock-Down in Italian School. Sustainability 13: 5812. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13115812
    [42] Greco G, Fischetti F, Cataldi S, et al. (2019) Effects of Shotokan Karate on resilience to bullying in adolescents. J Hum Sport Exerc 14: 890-899. https://doi.org/10.14198/jhse.2019.14.Proc4.52
    [43] Rocca A, Aprea G, Surfaro G, et al. (2016) Prevention and treatment of peritoneal adhesions in patients affected by vascular diseases following surgery: a review of the literature. Open Med 11: 106-114. https://doi.org/10.1515/med-2016-0021
    [44] Latino F, Tafuri F (2024) Teaching/Learning processes and inclusion: psycho-pedagogical perspectives. Ital J Health Educ Sport Inclusive Didact 8. https://doi.org/10.32043/gsd.v8i1.1018
    [45] Cabeza R, Albert M, Belleville S, et al. (2018) Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat Rev Neurosci 19: 701-710. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2
    [46] Vazou S, Pesce C, Lakes K, et al. (2019) More than one road leads to Rome: A narrative review and meta-analysis of physical activity intervention effects on cognition in youth. Int J Sport Exerce Ps 17: 153-178. https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2016.1223423
    [47] Latino F, Saraiello E, Tafuri F (2023) Outdoor Physical Activity: A Training Method for Learning in an Experiential and Innovative Way. J Phys Educ Sport (JPES) 23: 1852-1860.
    [48] Cataldi S, Latino F, Greco G, et al. (2019) Multilateral training improves physical fitness and fatigue perception in cancer patients. J Hum Sport Exerc 14: 910-920. https://doi.org/10.14198/jhse.2019.14.Proc4.54
    [49] Dishman RK, Berthoud HR, Booth FW, et al. (2006) Neurobiology of exercise. Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.) 14: 345-356. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2006.46
    [50] Mazzeo F, Monda M, Messina G, et al. (2016) Doping in Italy: An analysis of its spread in ten years. Biol Med 8: 1. https://doi.org/10.4172/0974-8369.1000263
    [51] Muscogiuri G, Palomba S, Caggiano M, et al. (2016) Low 25 (OH) vitamin D levels are associated with autoimmune thyroid disease in polycystic ovary syndrome. Endocrine 53: 538-542. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12020-015-0745-0
    [52] Guerra G, Testa D, Montagnani S, et al. (2014) Surgical management of pleomorphic adenoma of parotid gland in elderly patients: role of morphological features. Int J Surg 12: S12-S16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2014.08.391
    [53] Corvino A, Pignata S, Campanino MR, et al. (2020) Thyroglossal duct cysts and site-specific differential diagnoses: imaging findings with emphasis on ultrasound assessment. J Ultrasound 23: 139-149. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40477-020-00433-2
    [54] Wassenaar TM, Williamson W, Johansen-Berg H, et al. (2020) A critical evaluation of systematic reviews assessing the effect of chronic physical activity on academic achievement, cognition and the brain in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phy 17: 79. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-020-00959-y
    [55] Morsanuto S, Peluso Cassese F, Tafuri F, et al. (2023) Outdoor Education, Integrated Soccer Activities, and Learning in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Project Aimed at Achieving the Sustainable Development Goals of the 2030 Agenda. Sustainability 15: 13456. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151813456
    [56] Stillman CM, Cohen J, Lehman ME, et al. (2016) Mediators of Physical Activity on Neurocognitive Function: A Review at Multiple Levels of Analysis. Front Hum Neurosci 10: 626. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00626
    [57] Álvarez-Bueno C, Hillman CH, Cavero-Redondo I, et al. (2020) Aerobic fitness and academic achievement: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Sport Sci 38: 582-589. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2020.1720496
    [58] Erickson KI, Hillman C, Stillman CM, et al. (2019) Physical Activity, Cognition, and Brain Outcomes: A Review of the 2018 Physical Activity Guidelines. Med Sci Sport Exer 51: 1242-1251. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0000000000001936
    [59] Mavilidi MF, Ruiter M, Schmidt M, et al. (2018) A narrative review of school-based physical activity for enhancing cognition and learning: The importance of relevancy and integration. Front Psychol 9: 1-17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02079
    [60] Korol DL, Gold PE, Scavuzzo CJ (2013) Use it and boost it with physical and mental activity. Hippocampus 23: 1125-1135. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22197
    [61] Di Liegro CM, Schiera G, Proia P, et al. (2019) Physical Activity and Brain Health. Genes 10: 720. https://doi.org/10.3390/genes10090720
    [62] Lu B, Nagappan G, Lu Y (2014) BDNF and synaptic plasticity, cognitive function, and dysfunction. Neurotrophic Factors. Handbook of Experimental Pharmacology. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer 223-250. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-45106-5_9
    [63] Ferris LT, Williams JS, Shen CL (2007) The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Med Sci Sport Exer 39: 728-734. https://doi.org/10.1249/mss.0b013e31802f04c7
    [64] Cotman CW, Engesser-Cesar C (2002) Exercise enhances and protects brain function. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 30: 75-79. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003677-200204000-00006
    [65] Bhattacharjee J, Mohammad S, Goudreau AD, et al. (2021) Physical activity differentially regulates VEGF, PlGF, and their receptors in the human placenta. Physiol Rep 9: e14710. https://doi.org/10.14814/phy2.14710
    [66] Voss MW, Heo S, Prakash RS, et al. (2013) The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Hum Brain Mapp 34: 2972-2985. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22119
    [67] Van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C, et al. (2005) Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J Neurosci 25: 8680-8685. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-05.2005
    [68] Fisher A, Boyle JM, Paton JY, et al. (2011) Effects of a physical education intervention on cognitive function in young children: randomized controlled pilot study. BMC Pediatr 11: 97. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2431-11-97
    [69] Ferris LT, Williams JS, Shen CL (2007) The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Med Sci Sport Exer 39: 728-734. https://doi.org/10.1249/mss.0b013e31802f04c7
    [70] Latino F, Tafuri F (2024) Physical Activity and Cognitive Functioning. Medicina 60: 216. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60020216
    [71] Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, et al. (2011) Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. P Natl Acad Sci USA 108: 3017-3022. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1015950108
    [72] Tafuri F, Latino F (2024) School Medical Service: Strategies to Promote Psycho-Physiological Well-Being. Pediatr Rep 16: 214-231. https://doi.org/10.3390/pediatric16010019
    [73] Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson KI, Voss MW, et al. (2013) The effects of physical activity on functional MRI activation associated with cognitive control in children: a randomized controlled intervention. Front Hum Neurosci 7: 72. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00072
    [74] Chaddock L, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH, et al. (2011) A review of the relation of aerobic fitness and physical activity to brain structure and function in children. J Int Neuropsych Soc 17: 975-985. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000567
    [75] Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM, et al. (2014) Effects of the FITkids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics 134: e1063-e1071. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-3219
    [76] Jacqueline DG, Famelia R, Bakhtiar S (2014) Future directions in physical education & sport: Developing fundamental motor competence in the early years is paramount to lifelong physical activity. Asian Soc Sci 10: 44-54. https://doi.org/10.5539/ass.v10n5p44
    [77] Herrmann C, Gerlach E, Seelig H (2015) Development and validation of a test instrument for the assessment of basic motor competencies in primary school. Meas Phys Educ Exerc 19: 80-90. https://doi.org/10.1080/1091367X.2014.998821
    [78] Valtr L, Psotta R, Abdollahipour R (2016) Gender differences in performance of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-test in adolescents. Acta Gymnica 46: 155-161. https://doi.org/10.5507/ag.2016.017
    [79] Jelovčan G, Zurc J (2016) Preschool children's results in movement ABC test: Differences between girls and boys in movement deficit. Ann Kinesiol 7: 3-19.
    [80] Farì G, Di Paolo S, Ungaro D, et al. (2021) The Impact of COVID-19 on Sport and Daily Activities in an Italian Cohort of Football School Children. Int J Athl Ther Trai 26: 274-278. https://doi.org/10.1123/ijatt.2020-0066
    [81] Kokštejn J, Musálek M, Tufano JJ (2017) Are sex differences in fundamental motor skills uniform throughout the entire preschool period?. PloS One 12: e0176556. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0176556
    [82] Junaid KA, Fellowes S (2006) Gender differences in the attainment of motor skills on the movement assessment battery for children. Phys Occup Ther Pedi 26: 5-11. https://doi.org/10.1080/J006v26n01_02
    [83] Matarma T, Lagström H, Löyttyniemi E, et al. (2020) Motor skills of 5-year-old children: gender differences and activity and family correlates. Percept Motor Skill 127: 367-385. https://doi.org/10.1177/0031512519900732
    [84] Vega Ramírez L (2024) Breakdown of motor skills in primary school children: weight status and gender. J Phys Educ Sport 24: 560-566.
  • Reader Comments
  • © 2024 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(1073) PDF downloads(53) Cited by(0)

Figures and Tables

Tables(2)

Other Articles By Authors

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog