The growing interest in the study of morality has led to the birth of a new discipline in the field of moral philosophy called Neuroethics, a multidisciplinary approach that aims to combine philosophy and neuroscience. In this editorial, we explored the relevance of clinical models affected by neurological/psychiatric disorders to learn more about mechanisms sub-serving ethical behaviour at neural and cognitive level.
Citation: Carmelo M Vicario, Chiara Lucifora. Neuroethics: what the study of brain disorders can tell about moral behavior[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(4): 543-547. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021029
[1] | Fiammetta Iannuzzo, Silvia Crudo, Gianpaolo Antonio Basile, Fortunato Battaglia, Carmenrita Infortuna, Maria Rosaria Anna Muscatello, Antonio Bruno . Efficacy and safety of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques for the treatment of nicotine addiction: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(3): 212-225. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024014 |
[2] | Ronald Kamoga, Godfrey Zari Rukundo, Samuel Kalungi, Wilson Adriko, Gladys Nakidde, Celestino Obua, Johnes Obongoloch, Amadi Ogonda Ihunwo . Vagus nerve stimulation in dementia: A scoping review of clinical and pre-clinical studies. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(3): 398-420. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024024 |
[3] | AB Segarra, I Prieto, M Martínez-Cañamero, Manuel Ramírez-Sánchez . Is there a link between depression, neurochemical asymmetry and cardiovascular function?. AIMS Neuroscience, 2020, 7(4): 360-372. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2020022 |
[4] | Akshay Reddy, Mohammad Reza Hosseini, Aashay Patel, Ramy Sharaf, Vishruth Reddy, Arman Tabarestani, Brandon Lucke-Wold . Deep brain stimulation, lesioning, focused ultrasound: update on utility. AIMS Neuroscience, 2023, 10(2): 87-108. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2023007 |
[5] | Natalie Ceballos, Shobhit Sharma . Risk and Resilience: The Role of Brain-derived Neurotrophic Factor in Alcohol Use Disorder. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(4): 398-432. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.4.398 |
[6] | Mani Pavuluri, Kelley Volpe, Alexander Yuen . Nucleus Accumbens and Its Role in Reward and Emotional Circuitry: A Potential Hot Mess in Substance Use and Emotional Disorders. AIMS Neuroscience, 2017, 4(1): 52-70. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.1.52 |
[7] | Daniel Chimuanya Ugwuanyi, Tochukwu Florence Sibeudu, Chidmma Precious Irole, Michael Promise Ogolodom, Chukwudi Thaddeus Nwagbara, Adaobi Maryann Ibekwe, Awajimijan Nathaniel Mbaba . Evaluation of common findings in brain computerized tomography (CT) scan: A single center study. AIMS Neuroscience, 2020, 7(3): 311-318. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2020017 |
[8] | Timothy J. Ricker . The Role of Short-term Consolidation in Memory Persistence. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(4): 259-279. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.4.259 |
[9] | Andrea J. Rapkin, Steven M. Berman, Edythe D. London . The Cerebellum and Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder. AIMS Neuroscience, 2014, 1(2): 120-141. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.2.120 |
[10] | Yinghua Zhang, Xinyue Wei, Wenhao Zhang, Feng Jin, Wenbo Cao, Mingjin Yue, Saijun Mo . The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism serves as a potential marker of body weight in patients with psychiatric disorders. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(2): 188-202. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024012 |
The growing interest in the study of morality has led to the birth of a new discipline in the field of moral philosophy called Neuroethics, a multidisciplinary approach that aims to combine philosophy and neuroscience. In this editorial, we explored the relevance of clinical models affected by neurological/psychiatric disorders to learn more about mechanisms sub-serving ethical behaviour at neural and cognitive level.
The growing interest of philosophers to neuroscience has promoted the birth of a new discipline in the field of ethics called Neuroethics.
The innovative side of this discipline is the adoption of neuroscientific methods to explain how, and which neural processes subserve ethical reasoning. However, it should be clarified that the mission of Neuroethics is not limited to the investigation of neurobiological correlates of moral behavior, but it also includes questions about the moral implications of neuroscientific methods. In this regard, it is useful the terminological distinction by Adina Roskies between “Neurosciences of Ethics” and “Ethics of Neurosciences” [1].
Regarding the “Neuroscience of Ethics”, some interesting insight can be derived from the study of clinical populations affected by neurological and/or psychiatric disorders. A paradigmatic case, which is probably the first one reported in the literature, was described by Harrlow [2] with the patient Phineas Gage. As result of a work accident, which made this patient victim of a severe brain injury involving the medial and orbital regions of the frontal lobe, Phineas Gage had become an impulsive, violent and profane man. Damasio [3] suggested that a lesion of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) leads to the loss of the influence of ethics principles and the emotional appraisal typically involved in the evaluation of moral outcome such as the discrimination of good from bad. Moreover, patients affected by a damage of the vmPFC, were unable to correct or control harmful behaviors and/or unusual reactions while facing moral outcomes, such as those provided through the “Iowa Gambling” Task [4], a psychological task developed to measure “real world” decision-making deficits in patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex [5]. According to Damasio [3], it is not the reasoning ability of these patients that is affected but their emotions, intended as somatic markers used by the brain to quickly and unconsciously filter options with important positive or negative emotional consequences.
If the case of Phineas Gage highlights the importance of the frontal lobe in moral behavior, subsequent studies involving healthy participants through the use of neurophysiological techniques, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging and non-invasive brain stimulation methods, have revealed a wider and much more complex neural network. Among these regions it should be mentioned the cingulate cortex, a neural structure considered important in mediating the conflict between the emotional and rational component of moral reasoning [6]; the insula, a neural structure of central importance in the elaboration of interoceptive states (e.g. [7],[8]), which seems to be involved in the elaboration of the affective component of the sense of iniquity [6],[9]; and the basal ganglia such as the subthalamic nucleus, which is involved in the evaluation of morally related conflicting situations [10].
Turning back to the relevance of clinical models in the field of Neuroethics, it should be mentioned the case of movement disorders such as Parkinson's syndrome, Huntington's chorea, and Tourette's syndrome, which are characterized by a reduced sensitivity towards ethical violations (for a review see [11],[12],[13]. Moreover, an important contribute comes from the examination of psychiatric syndromes such as obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) [11],[14] and Depression [15], which are characterized by a high sensitivity to ethical violations. It is interesting to note that all syndromes mentioned above are affected by similar anatomo-functional alteration of neural structures (Insula, Cingulate Cortex, Basal Ganglia) involved in moral processing (see [11] for a review). Furthermore, for all these syndromes it is documented an altered representation/perception of disgust (from core disgust to social disgust).
Disgust can be explained as a negative emotion towards specific stimuli that leads to avoid them [16]. Since several years there is a debate about a possible relationship between disgust and morality. Pizarro and colleagues [17] developed three different principles to explain this relationship. In the first case, it is possible to speak about the experience of disgust as a consequence of moral disapproval (e.g., [18]). In the second case, disgust becomes an instrument to prime (amplify) moral rejection. For example, it was shown that inducing disgust leads people to have harsher moral judgments (e.g.,[19]). Moreover, the lower the disgust sensitivity the lower the disapproval of ethical violations (e.g., [20]). In the third case, disgust is understood as an engine to influence the judgment of morally neutral acts. According to the second principle, in Parkinson's Syndrome [21],[22] or Huntington's chorea [23],[24], the sensitivity to core disgust is reduced compared to controls. On the other hand, a higher sensitivity to disgust has been reported in patients affected by OCD [25] and depression (e.g.,[26]).
In Tourette's syndrome, we recently documented higher disgust sensitivity compared to controls [13]. This contrasts the greater tolerance of unethical behaviors in TS adolescents compared to controls, as the general literature which documents higher moral disapproval in individuals with higher disgust sensitivity [20],[27]. However, the reduced moral disapproval of ethical violations in TS is consistent with prior evidence of increased impulsivity in this clinical population [28], which is associated with reduced moral disapproval in healthy humans [29].
The evidence provided by the study of ethical behavior in clinical models supports the neo-sentimentalist perspective, which postulates a close relationship between disgust and morality (e.g.,[30]). However, it should be clarified that a distinction exists between “emotional appraisal” and “utilitarian appraisal” in the judgment of moral outcomes [31]. From a critical analysis [9] of the fMRI study by Hutcherson et al. [31], it was outlined that the neural circuits classically involved in the processing of disgust only overlap those involved in the “emotional appraisal” of moral outcomes. On the other hand, the neural circuit reported in response to “utilitarian appraisal” of moral topics is clearly independent from the one associated with disgust processing. This suggests that the influence of disgust on morality may be mainly involved in (if not limited to) the emotional evaluation of moral outcomes, instead of being considered the general root of morality.
In summary, these results provide a contribute to the current debate between the neosentimentalist and rationalist perspective of morality by suggesting that the influence of disgust on ethics behavior is related with the affective appraisal of this human experience.
In conclusion, the study of ethics through clinical models affected by neural disorders and, more generally, through the adoption of neuroscientific methods represents a valuable approach to learn more about the origin of the noblest faculty of the human being.
[1] |
Roskies A (2002) Neuroethics for the new millennium. Neuron 35: 21-23. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00763-8
![]() |
[2] |
Harlow JM (1848) Passage of an iron rod through the head. Boston Med Surg J 39: 389-393. doi: 10.1056/NEJM184812130392001
![]() |
[3] | Damasio A (1994) L'errore di Cartesio. Emozione, ragione, cervello umano Milano: Adelphi. |
[4] | Tranel D, Damasio H (1991) Somatic Markers and the Guidance of Behaviour–Theory and Preliminary Testing. Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction, New York 217-229. |
[5] |
Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, et al. (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50: 7-15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3
![]() |
[6] |
Fumagalli M, Priori A (2012) Functional and clinical neuroanatomy of morality. Brain 135: 2006-2021. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr334
![]() |
[7] |
Vicario CM, Rafal RD, Martino D, et al. (2017) Core, social and moral disgust are bounded: A review on behavioral and neural bases of repugnance in clinical disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 80: 185-200. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.008
![]() |
[8] | Vicario CM, Rafal RD, di Pellegrino G, et al. (2020) Indignation for moral violations suppresses the tongue motor cortex: preliminary TMS evidence. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci . |
[9] |
Vicario CM (2016) Emotional Appraisal of Moral Dilemmas: What Neuroimaging Can Tell about the Disgust-Morality Link. J Neurosci 36: 263-264. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3483-15.2016
![]() |
[10] |
Fumagalli M, Giannicola G, Rosa M, et al. (2011) Conflict-dependent dynamic of subthalamic nucleus oscillations during moral decisions. Soc Neurosci 6: 243-256. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.515148
![]() |
[11] |
Vicario CM, Rafal RD, Borgomaneri S, et al. (2017) Pictures of disgusting foods and disgusted facial expressions suppress the tongue motor cortex. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 12: 352-362. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw129
![]() |
[12] |
Vicario CM, Martino D (2018) Social communication in Tourette syndrome: a glimpse at the contribution of the insula and the prefrontal cortex. Brain 141: e58. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy140
![]() |
[13] |
Vicario CM, Gulisano M, Maugeri N, et al. (2021) Moral Decision-Making in Adolescents with Tourette Syndrome. Mov Disord 36: 2205-2206. doi: 10.1002/mds.28705
![]() |
[14] |
Vicario CM (2013) Morality and disgust: insights from obsessive compulsive disorder. Front Psychiatry 3: 113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00113
![]() |
[15] |
Pulcu E, Zahn R, Elliott R (2013) The role of self-blaming moral emotions in major depression and their impact on social-economical decision making. Front Psychol 4: 310. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00310
![]() |
[16] | Ekman P, Friesen WV (1975) Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial cues Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. |
[17] |
Pizarro D, Inbar Y, Helion C (2011) On disgust and moral judgment. Emotion Rev 3: 267-268. doi: 10.1177/1754073911402394
![]() |
[18] |
Chapman HA, Kim DA, Susskind JM, et al. (2009) In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science 323: 1222-1226. doi: 10.1126/science.1165565
![]() |
[19] |
Wheatley T, Haidt J (2005) Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychol Sci 16: 780-784. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01614.x
![]() |
[20] |
Vicario CM, Rafal RD (2017) Relationship between body mass index and moral disapproval rating for ethical violations. Pers Indiv differ 104: 8-11. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.027
![]() |
[21] |
Bowers D, Miller K, Mikos A, et al. (2006) Startling facts about emotion in Parkinson's disease: blunted reactivity to aversive stimuli. Brain 129: 3356-3365. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl301
![]() |
[22] |
Ille R, Wabnegger A, Schwingenschuh P, et al. (2015) Role of disgust proneness in Parkinson's disease: avoxel-based morphometry study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 21: 314-317. doi: 10.1017/S135561771500017X
![]() |
[23] |
Mitchell IJ, Heims H, Neville EA, et al. (2005) Huntington's disease patients show impaired perception of disgust inthe gustatory and olfactory modalities. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 17: 119-121. doi: 10.1176/jnp.17.1.119
![]() |
[24] |
Hayes CJ, Stevenson RJ, Coltheart M (2007) Disgust and Huntington's disease. Neuropsychologia 45: 1135-1151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.015
![]() |
[25] |
Tsao SD, McKay D (2004) Behavioral avoidance tests and disgust in contamination fears: distinctions from trait anxiety. Behav Res Ther 42: 207-216. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00119-0
![]() |
[26] |
Powell PA, Simpson J, Overton PG (2013) When disgust leads to dysphoria: a three wave longitudinal study assessing the temporal relationship between self-disgust anddepressive symptoms. Cogn Emot 27: 900-913. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.767223
![]() |
[27] |
Vicario CM, Kuran KA, Rogers R, et al. (2018) The effect of hunger and satiety in the judgment of ethical violations. Brain Cogn 125: 32-36. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.05.003
![]() |
[28] |
Wright A, Rickards H, Cavanna AE (2012) Impulse-control disorders in gilles de la tourette syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 24: 16-27. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.10010013
![]() |
[29] |
Lucifora C, Martino G, Curcuruto A, et al. (2021) How Self-Control Predicts Moral Decision Making: An Exploratory Study on Healthy Participants. Int J Environ Res Public Health 18: 3840. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18073840
![]() |
[30] |
Haidt J (2001) The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol Rev 108: 814-834. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814
![]() |
[31] |
Hutcherson CA, Montaser-Kouhsari L, Woodward J, et al. (2015) Emotional and Utilitarian Appraisals of Moral Dilemmas Are Encoded in Separate Areas and Integrated in Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. J Neurosci 35: 12593-12605. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3402-14.2015
![]() |
1. | Mario F. Mendez, The Implications of Moral Neuroscience for Brain Disease: Review and Update, 2023, 1543-3641, 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000344 | |
2. | Laura Culicetto, Francesca Ferraioli, Chiara Lucifora, Alessandra Falzone, Gabriella Martino, Giuseppe Craparo, Alessio Avenanti, Carmelo Mario Vicario, Disgust as a transdiagnostic index of mental illness: A narrative review of clinical populations, 2023, 87, 0025-9284, 53, 10.1521/bumc.2023.87.suppA.53 | |
3. | Francesco Tortora, Abed L. Hadipour, Simone Battaglia, Alessandra Falzone, Alessio Avenanti, Carmelo M. Vicario, The Role of Serotonin in Fear Learning and Memory: A Systematic Review of Human Studies, 2023, 13, 2076-3425, 1197, 10.3390/brainsci13081197 | |
4. | Carmelo Mario Vicario, Nicoletta Maugeri, Chiara Lucifora, Adriana Prato, Massimo Mucciardi, Davide Martino, Renata Rizzo, Evidence for a dissociation between moral reasoning and moral decision‐making in Tourette syndrome, 2024, 18, 1748-6645, 217, 10.1111/jnp.12350 | |
5. | YS Filatova, IA Zolotova, Neuroethics in medicine. Pressing issues, 2024, 10.24075/medet.2024.029 | |
6. | Carmelo M. Vicario, Chiara Lucifora, Giuseppe Craparo, Paola Magnano, Gabriella Martino, Editorial: The social brain: new insights from social, clinical, and biological psychology, 2025, 16, 1664-1078, 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1552456 |