Citation: Hyein Cho, Wei Dou, Zaviera Reyes, Mark W. Geisler, Ezequiel Morsella. The reflexive imagery task: An experimental paradigm for neuroimaging[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(2): 97-115. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.2.97
[1] | Di Lollo V, Enns JT, Rensink RA (2000) Competition for consciousness among visual events: The psychophysics of reentrant visual pathways. J Exp Psychol Gen 129: 481–507. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.4.481 |
[2] | Mathewson KE, Gratton G, Fabiani M, et al. (2009) To see or not to see: Prestimulus alpha phase predicts visual awareness. J Neurosci 29: 2725–2732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3963-08.2009 |
[3] | Crick F, Koch C (2003) A framework for consciousness. Nat Neurosci 6: 119–126. doi: 10.1038/nn0203-119 |
[4] | Most SB, Scholl BJ, Clifford ER, et al. (2005) What you see is what you set: Sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness. Psychol Rev 112: 217–242. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.217 |
[5] | Merker B (2007) Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behav Brain Sci 30: 63–134. |
[6] | Freeman WJ (2006) William James on consciousness, revisited. Chaos Complexity Lett 1: 17–42. |
[7] | Köhler W (1948) Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. Q Rev Biol. |
[8] | Searle JR (2000) Consciousness. Annu Rev Neurosci 23: 557–578. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.557 |
[9] | Gazzaley A, D'Esposito M (2007) Unifying prefrontal cortex function: Executive control, neural networks and top-down modulation, In: Miller B, Cummings J (Ed.), The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders, New York: Guilford Press, 187–206. |
[10] | Ezequiel M, Godwin CA, Jantz TK, et al. (2015) Homing in on consciousness in the nervous system: An action-based synthesis. Behav Brain Sci 39: 1–17. |
[11] | Ach N (1905/1951) Determining tendencies: Awareness, In: Rapaport D (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought, New York: Columbia University Press, 15–38. |
[12] | Freud S, Brill AA (1938) The basic writings of Sigmund Freud. New York: Modern Library. |
[13] | Helmholtz Hv (1856/1925) Treatise of physiological optics: Concerning the perceptions in general, In: T. Shipley (Ed.), Classics in psychology, New York: Philosophy Library, 79–127. |
[14] | James W (1950) The principles of psychology. Am J Psychol 2: 761. |
[15] | Miller NE (1959) Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: Extensions to conflict behavior, motivation, and social learning, In: Koch S (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science, Vol. 2, New York: McGraw-Hill, 196–292. |
[16] | Wegner DM (1990) White bears and other unwanted thoughts. Suppr Obsession Psychol Mental Control. |
[17] | Lashley KS (1956) Cerebral organization and behavior, In: Proceedings of the association for research in nervouse and mental diseases, 36: 1–18. |
[18] | Miller GA (1962) Psychology: The science of mental life. Pelican Books. |
[19] | Fodor JA (1983) Modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT press. |
[20] | Morsella E (2005) The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory. Psychol Rev 112: 1000–1021. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.1000 |
[21] | Ohman A, Mineka S (2001) Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol Rev 108: 483–522. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483 |
[22] | Bargh JA, Morsella E (2008) The unconscious mind. Perspect Psychol Sci 3: 73–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00064.x |
[23] | Allen AK, Wilkins K, Gazzaley A, et al. (2013) Conscious thoughts from reflex-like processes: A new experimental paradigm for consciousness research. Conscious Cognition 22: 1318–1331. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.007 |
[24] | Bhangal S, Cho H, Geisler MW, et al. (2016) The prospective nature of voluntary action: Insights from the reflexive imagery task. Rev Gen Psychol 20: 101–117. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000071 |
[25] | Stroop JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol Gen 121: 15–23. |
[26] | Gollwitzer PM (1999) Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. Am Psychol 54: 493–503. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493 |
[27] | Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem 6: 174–215. |
[28] | Cho H, Godwin CA, Geisler MW, et al. (2014) Internally generated conscious contents: Interactions between sustained mental imagery and involuntary subvocalizations. Front Psychol 5: 1445. |
[29] | Merrick C, Farnia M, Jantz TK, et al. (2015) External control of the stream of consciousness: Stimulus-based effects on involuntary thought sequences. Conscious Cognition 33: 217–225. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.014 |
[30] | Levelt WJM (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. |
[31] | Cho H, Zarolia P, Gazzaley A, et al. (2016) Involuntary symbol manipulation (Pig Latin) from external control: Implications for thought suppression. Acta Psychol 166: 37–41. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.03.004 |
[32] | Miller BL, Cummings JL (2007) The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders, second edition. New York: Guilford Press. |
[33] | Block N (2007) Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behav Brain Sci 30: 481–548. |
[34] | Morsella E, Wilson LE, Berger CC, et al. (2009) Subjective aspects of cognitive control at different stages of processing. Atten Percept Psychophysics 71: 1807–1824. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.8.1807 |
[35] | Mason MF, Norton MI, Horn JDV, et al. (2007) Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315: 393–345. doi: 10.1126/science.1131295 |
[36] | Mcvay JC, Kane MJ (2010) Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psycho Bull 136: 198–207. |
[37] | Mitchell JP, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM, et al. (2010) Separating sustained from transient aspects of cognitive control during thought suppression. Psychol Sci 18: 292–297. |
[38] | Pasley BN, David SV, Mesgarani N, et al. (2012) Reconstructing speech from human auditory cortex. PLoS Biol 10: e1001251. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001251 |
[39] | Wyland CL, Kelley WM, Macrae CN, et al. (2003) Neural correlates of thought suppression. Neuropsychologia 41: 1863–1867. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.08.001 |
[40] | Cushing D, Morsella E (2016) The polymodal role of consciousness in adaptive action selection: A paradigm for neuroimaging, In: Poster presented at the Annual Convention of the Society for Cognitive Neuroscience, New York. |
[41] | Wegner DM (1994) Ironic processes of thought control. Psychol Rev 101: 34–52. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.34 |
[42] | Bhangal S, Merrick C, Morsella E (2015) Ironic effects as reflexive responses: Evidence from word frequency effects on involuntary subvocalizations. Acta Psychol 159: 33–40. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.04.006 |
[43] | Bhangal S, Allen AK, Geisler MW, et al. (2016) Conscious contents as reflexive processes: Evidence from the habituation of high-level cognitions. Conscious Cognition 41: 177–188. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.012 |
[44] | Merrick C, Cho H, Morsella E (2014) The reflexive imagery task: Unintentional imagery despite extensive training and voluntary set selection. Unpublished Manuscript, San Francisco State University. |
[45] | Allen AK, Krisst L, Montemayor C, et al. (2016) Entry of involuntary conscious contents from ambiguous images. Psychol Conscious Theory Res Pract 3: 326–337. doi: 10.1037/cns0000095 |
[46] | Rassin E (2005) Thought suppression. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier. |
[47] | Miller EK (2000) The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nat Rev Neurosci 1: 59–65. doi: 10.1038/35036228 |
[48] | Munakata Y, Herd SA, Chatham CH, et al. (2011) A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends Cognit Sci 15: 453–459. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.011 |
[49] | Anderson MC, Ochsner KN, Kuhl B, et al. (2004) Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science 303: 232–235. doi: 10.1126/science.1089504 |
[50] | Giuliano RJ, Wicha NY (2010) Why the white bear is still there: Electrophysiological evidence for ironic semantic activation during thought suppression. Brain Res 1316: 62–74. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.041 |
[51] | Gazzaley A, Nobre AC (2011) Top-down modulation: Bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends Cognit Sci 16: 129–135. |
[52] | Cohen JD, Dunbar K, McClelland JL (1990) On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychol Rev 97: 332–361. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.332 |
[53] | Brown JW, Braver TS (2005) Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science 307: 1118–1121. doi: 10.1126/science.1105783 |
[54] | Botvinick MM (2007) Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognit Affective Behav Neurosci 7: 356–366. |
[55] | Levy BJ, Anderson MC (2002) Inhibitory processes and the control of memory retrieval. Trends Cognit Sci 6: 299–305. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01923-X |
[56] | Levy BJ, Anderson MC (2008) Individual differences in the suppression of unwanted memories: The executive deficit hypothesis. Acta Psychol 127: 623–635. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.12.004 |
[57] | Levy BJ, Anderson MC (2012) Purging of memories from conscious awareness tracked in the human brain. J Neurosci 32: 16785–16794. |
[58] | Hickok G (2009) Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. J Cognit Neurosci 21: 1229–1243. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21189 |
[59] | Schomers MR, Kirilina E, Weigand A, et al. (2015) Causal influence of articulatory motor cortex on comprehending single spoken words: TMS evidence. Cereb Cortex 25: 3894–3902. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu274 |
[60] | Buchsbaum BR (2013) The role of consciousness in the phonological loop: Hidden in plain sight. Front Psychol 4: 496. |
[61] | Buchsbaum BR, D'Esposito M (2008) The search for the phonological store: From loop to convolution. J Cognit Neurosci 20: 762–778. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20501 |
[62] | Dewitt I, Rauschecker JP (2012) Phoneme and word recognitionin the auditory ventral stream. Proc Nat Acad Sci U S A 109: 505–514. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113427109 |
[63] | Eggert GH, Wernicke C (1874/1977) Wernicke's works on aphasia: A sourcebook and review. Hague, Netherlands: Mouton. |
[64] | Gazzaniga MS, Ivry RB, Mangun GR (2009) Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind, 3rd edition. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. |
[65] | Peramunage D, Blumstein SE, Myers EB, et al. (2011) Phonological neighborhood effects in spoken word production: An fMRI study. J Cognit Neurosci 23: 593–603. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21489 |
[66] | Scott M (2013) Corollary discharge provides the sensory content of inner speech. Psychol Sci 24: 1824–1830. doi: 10.1177/0956797613478614 |
[67] | Ford JM, Gray M, Faustman WO, et al. (2005) Reduced gamma-band coherence to distorted feedback during speech when what you say is not what you hear. Int J Psychophysiology 57: 143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.002 |
[68] | Mahon BZ, Caramazza A (2008) A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. J Physiol Paris 102: 59–70. |
[69] | Gruber O, Gruber E, Falkai P (2005) Neural correlates of working memory deficits in schizophrenic patients. Ways to establish neurocognitive endophenotypes of psychiatric disorders. Radiologe 45: 153–160. |
[70] | Müller NG, Knight RT (2006) The functional neuroanatomy of working memory: Contributions of human brain lesion studies. Neurosci 139: 51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.09.018 |
[71] | Sato M, Baciu M, Loevenbruck H, et al. (2004) Multistable representation of speech forms: A functional MRI study of verbal transformations. NeuroImage 23: 1143–1151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.055 |
[72] | Vallar G, Corno M, Basso A (1992) Auditory and visual verbal short-term memory in aphasia. Cortex 28: 383–389. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80148-7 |
[73] | Pugh SR, Morsella E, Geisler MW (2014) Involuntary cognitions of positive and negative images: Behavioral consequences and EEG correlates. Poster presented at the Graduate Student Showcase at San Francisco State University, San Francisco. |
[74] | Fiedler K (2017) What constitutes strong psychological science? The (neglected) role of diagnosticity and a priori theorizing. Perspect Psychol Sci 12: 46–61. |
[75] | Nosek BA, Spies JR, Motyl M (2012) Scientific utopia II: Restructuring incentives and practices to promote truth over publishability. Perspect Psychol Sci 7: 615–631. doi: 10.1177/1745691612459058 |
[76] | Dehaene S (2014) Consciousness and the brain: Deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. New York: Viking. |
[77] | Koch C, Massimini M, Boly M, et al. (2016) Neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nat Rev Neurosci 17: 307–321. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.22 |
[78] | Cohen JD, Macwhinney B, Flatt M, et al. (1993) PsyScope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 25: 257–271. doi: 10.3758/BF03204507 |
[79] | Morsella E, Miozzo M (2002) Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 28: 555–563. |
[80] | Simon JR, Hinrichs JV, Craft JL (1970) Auditory S-R compatibility: Reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence. J Exp Psychol 86: 97–102. doi: 10.1037/h0029783 |
[81] | Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW, et al. (2006) A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Arch Intern Med 166: 1092–1097 doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092 |
[82] | Cho H (2015) Cognitive bias in involuntary cognitions toward negative-valenced stimuli in highly anxious/depressive groups. Masters Thesis, San Francisco State University. |
[83] | Bargh JA, Chartrand TL (2000) The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research, In: Reis HT, Judd CM (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology, Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 253–285. |
[84] | Wundt W (1902/1904) Principles of physiological psychology. Translated from the Fifth German Edition (1904) by Titchener EB, London: Swan Sonnenschein. |
[85] | Woodworth RS (1915) A revision of imageless thought. Psychol Rev 22: 1–27. doi: 10.1037/h0074506 |
[86] | Scullin MK, McDaniel MA, Einstein GO (2010) Control of cost in prospective memory: Evidence for spontaneous retrieval processes. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 36: 190–203. |
[87] | Schultz DP, Schultz SE (1996) A history of modern psychology, sixth edition. San Diego: Harbrace College Publishers. |
[88] | Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, DeWall N, et al. (2007) How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Pers Social Psychol Rev 11: 167–203. doi: 10.1177/1088868307301033 |
[89] | Firestone C, Scholl BJ (2016) Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for "top-down" effects. Behav Brain Sci 39: 1–77. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14001356 |