Research article Special Issues

Complexity and Autism Spectrum Disorder: Exploring Hysteresis in A Grasping Task

  • In the current paper, we explored the presence of hysteresis in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Hysteresis is a complexity flag, known to shed light on the dynamics of nonlinear systems. We chose a task that elicits hysteresis in typical development and carried it out with children with ASD. Specifically, children were asked to lift cubes that got increasingly bigger and then smaller (or vice versa). Smaller cubes could be lifted with one hand, whereas bigger cubes required two hands to be lifted. Thus, the change in cube size forced participants to switch their grasping patterns (from one hand to two vs. from two hands to one). The dependent variable was the transition point at which the switch in lifting patterns happened. Results show hysteresis for ASD, to the same degree found for typically developing children (Experiment 1). However, when a social component was added to the task (children had to hand the cubes to the experimenter), a clear difference was found between diagnostic groups (Experiment 2): While typically developing children still demonstrated hysteresis in their grasping, ASD participants no longer did so. It appears that the coordination dynamics of a motor task changed when children with ASD were asked to interact with the experimenter. We discuss the extent to which the analysis of coordination dynamics provides a unique window into understanding ASD.

    Citation: Joseph L. Amaral, Veronica Romero, Heidi Kloos, Michael J. Richardson. Complexity and Autism Spectrum Disorder: Exploring Hysteresis in A Grasping Task[J]. AIMS Medical Science, 2017, 4(1): 113-130. doi: 10.3934/medsci.2017.1.113

    Related Papers:

  • In the current paper, we explored the presence of hysteresis in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Hysteresis is a complexity flag, known to shed light on the dynamics of nonlinear systems. We chose a task that elicits hysteresis in typical development and carried it out with children with ASD. Specifically, children were asked to lift cubes that got increasingly bigger and then smaller (or vice versa). Smaller cubes could be lifted with one hand, whereas bigger cubes required two hands to be lifted. Thus, the change in cube size forced participants to switch their grasping patterns (from one hand to two vs. from two hands to one). The dependent variable was the transition point at which the switch in lifting patterns happened. Results show hysteresis for ASD, to the same degree found for typically developing children (Experiment 1). However, when a social component was added to the task (children had to hand the cubes to the experimenter), a clear difference was found between diagnostic groups (Experiment 2): While typically developing children still demonstrated hysteresis in their grasping, ASD participants no longer did so. It appears that the coordination dynamics of a motor task changed when children with ASD were asked to interact with the experimenter. We discuss the extent to which the analysis of coordination dynamics provides a unique window into understanding ASD.


    加载中
    [1] American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, D.C.
    [2] Bressler SL, Kelso JAS (2016) Coordination Dynamics in Cognitive Neuroscience. Frontiers Neurosci 10: 397-397.
    [3] Kelso JAS, Dumas G, Tognoli E (2013) Outline of a general theory of behavior and brain coordination. Neural Networks 37: 120-131. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.09.003
    [4] Richardson MJ, Dale R, Marsh KL (2014) Complex Dynamical Systems in Social and Personality Psychology. In: Reis HT, Judd CM, editors. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [5] Van Orden GC, Kloos H, Wallot S (2011) Living in the pink: Intentionality, Wellbeing, and Complexity. In: Hooker CA, editor. Philosophy of Complex Systems: Handbook of the Philosophy of Science. Amsterdam: Elsevier. pp. 639-682.
    [6] Camazine S (2003) Self-organization in biological systems: Princeton University Press.
    [7] Dapretto M, Bjork EL (2000) The Development of Word Retrieval Abilities in the Second Year and its Relation to Early Vocabulary Growth. Child Development 71: 635-648. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00172
    [8] Barrett M (1995) Early lexical development. Handbook Child Language: 362-392.
    [9] Kloos H, Keen R (2005) An Exploration of Toddlers' Problems in a Search Task. Infancy 7: 7-34. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0701_3
    [10] Amaral JL, Collins S, Bohache KT, et al. (2012) Beyond the Black-and-White of Autism: How Cognitive Performance Varies with Context. InTech.
    [11] Rajendran G, Mitchell P (2007) Cognitive theories of autism. Development Rev 27: 224-260. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.02.001
    [12] Happé F, Ronald A, Plomin R (2006) Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nat Neurosci 9: 1218-1220. doi: 10.1038/nn1770
    [13] Fitzpatrick P, Frazier JA, Cochran DM, et al. (2016) Impairments of Social Motor Synchrony Evident in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Frontiers Psychology 7: 1323-1323.
    [14] Ozonoff S (1997) Components of executive function in autism and other disorders: Oxford University Press.
    [15] Guastello SJ, Liebovitch LS (2009) Introduction to nonlinear dynamics and complexity. In: troduction to nonlinear d, complexity SJG, Koopmans M et al., editors. Chaos and complexity in psychology: The theory of nonlinear dynamical systems. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press. pp. 1-40.
    [16] 16. Ewing JA (1882) On Effects of Retentiveness in the Magnetisation of Iron and Steel. (Preliminary Notice). Proceedings Royal Soc London 34: 39-45.
    [17] 17. Tuller B, Case P, Ding M, et al. (1994) The nonlinear dynamics of speech categorization. J Experiment Psychol Human Perception Performance 20: 3-16. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.1.3
    [18] Fitzpatrick P, Carello C, Schmidt RC, et al. (1994) Haptic and Visual Perception of an Affordance for Upright Posture. Ecological Psychology 6: 265-287. doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco0604_2
    [19] Frank TD, Richardson MJ, Lopresti-Goodman SM, et al. (2009) Order Parameter Dynamics of Body-scaled Hysteresis and Mode Transitions in Grasping Behavior. J Biological Physics 35: 127-147. doi: 10.1007/s10867-009-9133-4
    [20] Lopresti-Goodman SM, Richardson MJ, Baron RM, et al. (2009) Task Constraints on Affordance Boundaries. Motor Control 13: 69-83. doi: 10.1123/mcj.13.1.69
    [21] Savelsbergh G, van der Kamp J, Davis W, et al. (1999) Hysteresis in perceptual-movement coordination: na.
    [22] Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Baron RM (2007) Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. J Experiment Psychol Human Perception Performance 33: 845-859. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.845
    [23] van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP, Davis WE (1998) Body-scaled ratio as a control parameter for prehension in 5- to 9-year-old children. Development Psychobiology 33: 351-361.
    [24] Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, et al. (2012) Autism diagnostic observation schedule–2nd edition (ADOS-2). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Corporation.
    [25] Elliott CD (2007) Differential Ability Scales-Second Edition, Introductory and Technical Manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
    [26] Rosselli M, Ardila A (1993) Developmental norms for the wisconsin card sorting test in 5-to 12-year-old children. Clinical Neuropsycholog 7: 145-154. doi: 10.1080/13854049308401516
    [27] Staff P (2003) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Computer Version 4, Research Edition (WCST: CV4). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
    [28] Hock HS, Kelso JA, Schöner G (1993) Bistability and hysteresis in the organization of apparent motion patterns. J Experiment Psychol Human Perception Performance 19: 63-80. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.1.63
    [29] van Bers BMCW, Visser I, van Schijndel TJP, et al. (2011) The dynamics of development on the Dimensional Change Card Sorting task. Developmental Sci 14: 960-971. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01045.x
    [30] Kelso JS (1997) Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior: MIT press.
    [31] Fitzpatrick P, Diorio R, Richardson MJ, et al. (2013) Dynamical methods for evaluating the time-dependent unfolding of social coordination in children with autism. Frontiers Integrative Neurosci 7: 21-21.
    [32] Boucenna S, Narzisi A, Tilmont E, et al. (2014) Interactive Technologies for Autistic Children: A Review. Cognitive Computation 6: 722-740. doi: 10.1007/s12559-014-9276-x
    [33] Diehl JJ, Schmitt LM, Villano M, et al. (2012) The clinical use of robots for individuals with autism spectrum disorders: A critical review. Res Autism Spectrum Disorders 6: 249-262. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.05.006
    [34] Ozonoff S (1995) Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology 9: 491-500. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.9.4.491
    [35] Robins B, Dautenhahn K, Dubowski J (2006) Does appearance matter in the interaction of children with autism with a humanoid robot? Interaction Studies 7: 509-542. doi: 10.1075/is.7.3.16rob
    [36] Loveland KA (1991) Social Affordances and Interaction II: Autism and the Affordances of the Human Environment. Ecological Psychology 3: 99-119. doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco0302_3
  • Reader Comments
  • © 2017 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(5000) PDF downloads(1024) Cited by(1)

Article outline

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog