Citation: Barbara Tomasino, Dario Marin, Cinzia Canderan, Marta Maieron, Miran Skrap, Raffaella Ida Rumiati. Neuropsychological Patterns Following Lesions of the Anterior Insula in a Series of Forty Neurosurgical Patients[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2014, 1(3): 225-244. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.3.225
[1] | Mani Pavuluri, Amber May . I Feel, Therefore, I am: The Insula and Its Role in Human Emotion, Cognition and the Sensory-Motor System. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(1): 18-27. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.1.18 |
[2] | Mohammad Mofatteh . Neurosurgery and artificial intelligence. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(4): 477-495. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021025 |
[3] | Yasir Rehman, Cindy Zhang, Haolin Ye, Lionel Fernandes, Mathieu Marek, Andrada Cretu, William Parkinson . The extent of the neurocognitive impairment in elderly survivors of war suffering from PTSD: meta-analysis and literature review. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(1): 47-73. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021003 |
[4] | Jarrod Moss . Introduction to AIMS Neuroscience Special Issue “What Function Does the Anterior Insula Play in Human Cognition?”. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(3): 153-154. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.3.153 |
[5] | Margaret Jane Moore, Nele Demeyere . Neglect Dyslexia in Relation to Unilateral Visuospatial Neglect: A Review. AIMS Neuroscience, 2017, 4(4): 148-168. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.4.148 |
[6] | Paul G. Nestor, Toshiyuki Ohtani, James J. Levitt, Dominick T. Newell, Martha E. Shenton, Margaret Niznikiewicz, Robert W. McCarley . Prefrontal Lobe Gray Matter, Cognitive Control and Episodic Memory in Healthy Cognition. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(3): 338-355. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.3.338 |
[7] | Gianna Sepede, Francesco Gambi, Massimo Di Giannantonio . Insular Dysfunction in People at Risk for Psychotic Disorders. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(2): 66-70. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.2.66 |
[8] | Masatoshi Takita, Yumi Izawa-Sugaya . Neurocircuit differences between memory traces of persistent hypoactivity and freezing following fear conditioning among the amygdala, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(2): 195-211. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021010 |
[9] | Byron Bernal, Alfredo Ardila, Monica Rosselli . The Network of Brodmanns Area 22 in Lexico-semantic Processing: A Pooling-data Connectivity Study. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(3): 306-316. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.3.306 |
[10] | Mark Reed, Christopher Miller, Cortney Connor, Jason S. Chang, Forshing Lui . Fat droplets in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) spaces of the brain. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(4): 484-489. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024029 |
The insula cortex is an integrative area [1,2,3]. A functional parcelization of the insular sub-regions is indicated by tracer studies in animals [4,5] which represent our primary understanding of structural connections of the insula, and indicate the differentiation of an anterior and a posterior sectors. Except for some DTI studies on humans [3,6,7,8], parcelization studies about the structural connectivity of the human insula are lacking due to technical problems in reconstructing the pathways passing thought he insula. Tracing studies in non-human primates [9,10] evidenced that the anterior-basal sector, connected with limbic areas, is part of a frontal-entorhinal, piriform and olfactory cortex network, while the mid-dorsal insula receives input from the thalamic taste area [11]. The mid-posterior sector of the insula cortex, instead, is connected with somatosensory areas [12,13].
This reach connectivity confers to the insular cortex the status of a multimodal processing area. A recent quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies, addressing functional activations found in the insula [3], showed that the anterior-ventral insula and the right central region are activated by emotional-social tasks, the right central region by gustatory-olfactory stimuli, and the anterior-dorsal insula by cognitive tasks. In particular, attention-related tasks activated the anterior-dorsal insula bilaterally, and language-related tasks such as lexical decision or semantic judgments activated the anterior dorsal insula bilaterally, and speech activated the anterior dorsal insula bilaterally. Working memory too activated the anterior dorsal insula bilaterally, while the sensorimotor tasks mainly activated the mid-posterior insula. In the same study [3], a conjunction analysis showed that the portion of the insula cortex shared by social-emotional, gustatory-olfactory and cognitive tasks involved the anterior-dorsal insula.
Despite this functional selectivity found in sub-parts of the insular cortex, selective lesions affecting the insula alone are extremely rare. Therefore exploring which selective deficits are caused by lesions to sub-parts of the insula is not a simple exercise. For instance it has been shown that only 4 out of 4800 examined (0.0008%) patients with stroke had a lesion selectively involving the insula [14]. This indicates that neuropsychological descriptions of insular patients often include lesions extending beyond the insula. Similarly, other authors showed that out of 72 examined patients with lesions to the insular cortex, none had purely isolated infarction of the insula without involvement of surrounding areas [15]. These data are provided in the context of studies including stroke patients. Neurosurgical lesions, such as those due to low or high grade gliomas, are usually more selective than those due to stroke in that the areas involved in the lesion are less diffuse than the damaged areas involved in stroke lesions. However, also for patients with insular neurosurgical lesions, the interpretation of cognitive deficits might be done in the context of the complex interactions between the insula and its adjacent areas. The neurosurgical studies mainly report effects of cortical stimulation or effects of the medial temporal lobe epilepsy. For example, the intraoperative direct stimulation of the anterior insula led to gustatory and olfactory sensations [16]. Some authors [17] observed clinical responses in 10 out of 14 patients, and found a clear topography during stimulation of the insular cortex. In particular, viscerosensitive and visceromotor responses were evoked in the anterior insula, while somesthetic sensations were evoked by posterior insula stimulation. Other authors [18], in a PET study, compared patients with medial temporal lobe epilepsy who experienced visceral or emotional symptoms with those who did not. They found that emotional symptoms were correlated with hypometabolism in the anterior ipsilateral insula while visceral symptoms were correlated with hypometabolism of the posterior insula. In addition it has been shown that insular epilepsy can trigger gustatory hallucinations [19].
Studies reporting the cognitive profile of neurosurgical patients include a retrospective study of a series of 66 cases (19 right insula and 47 left insula) of surgery of insular nonenhancing gliomas indicating that language disorders were reported in the immediate postoperative phase in 11 patients (16.7 articulatory disorders in 1 patient, phonemic paraphasia without comprehension deficit in 8 patients, speech disorders with comprehension deficits in 2 patients). At a three-month follow up examination, speech disorders were reported in 3% of the patients [20]. In addition, it has been argued that the left insula can be removed without inducing definitive aphasia due to a preoperative language reshaping shown by fMRI [21]. In another study with 42 neurosurgical patients with low-grade insular glioma, immediately following resection 10 patients experienced articulatory disorders, while at a three-month post surgery examination there was no evidence of language disorders [22]. No indication as to whether the anterior- vs posterior- subpart of the insula was involved, can be found. These results however are in contrast with other studies indicating a role of the anterior insula in language processing [23,24]. Deficits in sound detection and auditory temporal processing have been described in insula patients [25], as well as Broca’s aphasia [26] apraxia of speech [27], repetition deficit [28] and reduced fluency [29]. The insula has afferent and efferent connections to the temporal lobes. In addition, because of its proximity to other areas involved in language processing its linguistic role should be taken with caution. When lesions were restricted to the left insula [14], aphasic symptoms were inconsistent. It is however unclear which language-related tasks were administered. Thus the exact role of insula in language processing remains unclear. A meta-analysis of expressive language tasks [30] showed that the left insula was involved, among other areas, in the network supporting speech production, and another recent meta-analysis showed that receptive language, expressive language and speech production activated very similar regions of the anterior insula, while activation related to perception of speech activated the left dorsal mid-insula [31].
Aside language, the clinical effects of insular damage are multiple [32] and involve autonomic, perceptual, somatosensory, vestibular systems, pain processing, stimuli awareness, emotion, mood and willed action, language, and memory processing. Similarly, other authors [33] reviewed the effects of insular lesions by focusing on emotional, perceptual, sensorimotor, and body awareness disorders. Damage to the left insula can cause deficit in assigning taste adjectives to smell in the absence of olfactory processing impairments [34]. Auditory agnosia [35,36] and auditory temporal processing deficits [25] might arise following insular infarction. Right insular lesions can cause neglect [37,38]. Patients with right insular lesions had a greater frequency of subjective anergia, tiredness, depression and other mood disorders [39].
In the present study we investigated the effects of lesions affecting mainly the anterior insula in a series of 22 patients with lesions to the LH and 18 patients with lesions involving the RH. The insula can be affected by both low and high grade gliomas. Due to its location and its functional integration role resection of the insula tumors has traditionally been viewed as hazardous [33]. In particular, their tendency to spread along the intricate network of afferent and efferent connections between the insula itself and the surrounding cortical areas, constitutes a major difficulty in reaching a gross total resection [20]. We studied patients in the pre-operative phase, and reported their neuropsychological profile and an analysis of their pre-surgical symptoms. Besides characterizing the patients’ lesions as involving the anterior-posterior insula, we also investigated the effect of lesions involving the left and the right hemisphere. In addition, a sub-group of patients (8 RH patients and 9 LH patients) underwent to the post-surgery follow-up examination.
Twenty two right-handed neurosurgical patients (7 female, mean age 47.76 ± 11.71 years, range 32-70 and mean education 13.23 ± 3.50 years, range 5-18), whose tumor involved the left insula, and 18 right-handed neurosurgical patients (7 female) whose tumor involved the right insula (mean age 45 ± 14.54, range 23-71 and mean education 12.05 ± 4.22, range 5-18) participated in the study. They were admitted to the A.O.S. Maria della Misericordia a few days before the beginning of the study. All participants were native Italian speakers, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported no history of psychiatric disease or drug abuse. All the patients participated in the study before surgery. For some of them (8 RH patients and 9 LH patients) a follow-up examination was also included. Conventional T2-weighted MR imaging revealed lesions measuring 95 ± 58.52 cc for LH patients (volume range: 28-228.3 cc) and 91.95 ± 62.17 cc for RH patients (range 13.90-290.40, see Table 1 for individual volumes) (P > 0.05, n.s.). Inclusion criteria were as follows: i) presence of unilateral lesions due to tumors involving the insular cortex; ii) excluded extra-perenchimal lesions as meningioma. The study was approved by the ethical committee and has therefore been performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. All participants signed the informed consent.
RH Patients | ||||||||||||
anxiety/fear/panic attacks (p.a.) | epigastric: stomach/nausea | Fainting sensation | sensation | paresthesia | olfactory | sweating sensation | gustatory | auditory | motor | cognitive | ||
P1 anxiety, fear, p.a. | P2 stomach nausea, | P1 | P2 leg | P18 strange sensation | P5 | P9 difficulty in recognizing taste | P17 confusion | P6 distraction | ||||
P2 anxiety | P3 stomach | P8 arm, face | P11 ointment odor | P10 bitter taste | P9 > sensibility to sounds | P11 confusion | ||||||
P4 p.a. | P7 epigastric | P12 arm, face | P15 gustatory hallucinations | P16 prosopoagnosia | ||||||||
P5 p.a, anxiety depression | P12 stomach | P15 left side of the body | P18 strange taste | |||||||||
P6 anxiety | P4 strange taste | |||||||||||
P12 pleasure and anxiety | ||||||||||||
P15 anxiety | ||||||||||||
LH patients | ||||||||||||
P1 p.a. | P6 senso di vomito | - | P10 hand, leg and head | - | - | P1 strong breaths from the nose as hot air | P16 bitter taste | - | P2 leg stiffening | P1 Phon par | ||
P10 anxiety | P9 intenso freddo allo stomaco | P16 right side of the body | P3 seizure arm and leg | P2 confusion | ||||||||
P11 p.a. | P18 hand, leg | P5 seizure arm and face | P3 speech arrest | |||||||||
P14 unpleasent | P22 hand | P13 face | P4 anomia | |||||||||
sensations, anxiety | and cheeck | |||||||||||
P18 hand stiffening | P5 diff to speak phon paraf | |||||||||||
P19 leg stiffening | P7 confusion | |||||||||||
P8 anomia | ||||||||||||
P13 diff speak | ||||||||||||
P14 anomia | ||||||||||||
P15 verbal paraphasia | ||||||||||||
P17 diff to speak | ||||||||||||
P18 articulation and anomia | ||||||||||||
P19 anomia and stm | ||||||||||||
P20anomia phon paraph |
A 3-T Philips Achieva whole-body scanner was used for the patients using a SENSE-Head-8 channel head coil and a custom-built head restrainer to minimize head movements. Magnetic resonance imaging was performed 6-10 days prior to craniotomy. High-resolution T2-weighted and post-gadolinium contrast T1-weighted anatomical MR images were acquired for use with the stereotactic surgical navigation system by using a T1-weighted 3D magnetization-prepared, rapid acquisition gradient-echo fast field echo (T1W_3D_TFE SENSE) pulse sequence (TR = 8.1007 ms, TE = 3.707 ms, FOV = 240.000 mm, 190 sagittal slices of 1 mm thickness, flip angle = 8°, voxel size: 1 × 1 × 1) and a T3-weighted 3D magnetization-prepared, rapid acquisition gradient-echo fast field echo (T2W_3D_TFE SENSE) pulse sequence (TR = 2500 ms, TE = 368.328 ms, FOV = 240.000 mm, 190 sagittal slices of 1 mm thickness, flip angle = 90°, voxel size: 1 × 1 × 1).
To determine the location of patients’ lesions we used the overlap method. MRIcron software http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricro/mricron/index.html) was used to draw the patients’ lesions on their T1 and T2 MRI scans, creating 40 ROIs which were normalized to the MNI space using the “Clinical Toolbox” (http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/CRNL/clinical-toolbox) for SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8/) (Figure 1). By using MRIcron software we created the overlay plot of all the LH patients and that of RH patients. In Figure 1 the lesion density bar shows that as many as 20/22 of the LH patients and 16/18 RH patients had a lesion involving the anterior insula. By using the probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps of the posterior insula derived by the anatomy toolbox [40] we created a ROI and we overlapped it (in violet) on the overlay plot of all the LH patients (Figure 1A) and that of RH (Figure 1B) patients. Figure 1 clearly shows that the lesions were anterior to the ROI comprising the posterior part of the insula (as shown bilaterally in violet). Lastly, the anatomical interpretation of the overlay plot of all the LH patients and that of RH patients has been made by using the Anatomy toolbox [40] which revealed that for both the LH and the RH patients the maximum density area overlapped with the insular cortex.
Particular attention was given to patients’ own description of pre-surgery symptoms. In particular the following categories were created: i) anxiety/panic and attacks/fear ii) gastric/nausea, iii) fainting sensations, iv) paresthesia, v) olfactory, vi) gustatory, vii) sweating, viii) auditory, ix) cognitive, x) sensations, and xi) motor. For the LH and RH group we then calculated the percentage of patients reporting symptoms involving the above mentioned categories.
Each patient was submitted to a neuropsychological battery before the MRI scanning. The neuropsychological evaluation for our LH patients included tests assessing spatio-temporal orientation, handedness (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [41], non-verbal intelligence [Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices [42], verbal short-term memory [43], praxis (buccofacial [44] and ideomotor [45], language comprehension (Token test [46]), noun naming and verb naming [47], phonological fluency [48], word and pseudoword reading, repetition and writing [47], phonological discrimination and lexical decision [47]. The neuropsychological evaluation of the RH patientss included tests assessing the spatio-temporal orientation, handedness (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [41]), non-verbal intelligence [42], spatial short-term memory, constructional apraxia [44], visuospatial planning [49], visuo-spatial ability [50], and attention [51].
We found that 38.88% of the RH patients reported anxiety/fear/panic attacks, 27.77% reported taste sensations (e.g, bitter taste, strange gustatory sensations), 22.22% gastric sensations (e.g, stomach/nausea), 22.22% paresthesia (e.g, leg, arm), 16.66% cognitive changes (e.g, attention, confusion), 11.11% olfactory sensations (e.g, ointment odor), 11.11% auditory sensations (e.g, increased sensibility, confusion) and 5.55% other sensations (e.g, sweating and feeling fainting). None reported motor symptoms or sensations (Figure 2).
The LH patients presented a different pattern. The 63.63% presented cognitive changes (e.g, phonologic paraphasia, speech arrest, anomia), 27.27% motor symptoms (e.g, seizures involving the arm or the leg, or mouth), 18.18% anxiety/fear/panic attacks, 18.18% paresthesia (e.g, hand, arm, face), 9% gastric sensations (e.g, stomach/nausea/cold sensations to the stomach), 4.5% general sensations (e.g, hot air while breathing from the nose) and gustatory sensations (e.g, bitter taste). None reported experience of olfactory, auditory sensation nor feeling fainting or sweating.
By comparing the LH and RH patients we found a significant difference for two categories, namely cognitive (χ2 = 8.93, P > 0.005) and motor symptoms (χ2y1 = 3.83, P > 0.05) with significantly higher percentage of LH patients reporting changes in the two categories than RH patients. All the others did not differ significantly between LH patients and RH patients (all Yates' P-value > 0.05, n.s.) (Figure 2 and Table 1).
A tight parallelism was found between the pattern that emerged in the pre-surgery symptoms analysis and the patients’ cognitive profile. In particular, we found that LH patients showed more cognitive deficits than RH patients (see Tables 2-3 and Figure 2B). With the exception of one case showing buccofacial apraxia in addition to reading and repetition deficit (P5) and one case showing a mild comprehension deficit at the Token test in addition to lexical decision deficit (P15) and two cases showing a lexical decision deficit (P13 and P17), 31.81% of the LH patients showed a noun naming deficit, the 36.26% showed a verb naming deficit and the 27.27% showed an impaired performance at the phonological fluency test. All the other tests were within the normal range (Table 2 and Figure 2B). By contrast, the RH patients, with the exception of one patient showing an impaired performance at the visuo-spatial planning test (Clock test) (P2), were within the normal range in performing all the tests (Table 3 and Figure 2B). This result fits with the pre-surgery symptoms analysis where the majority of the RH patients did not report changes or symptoms involving the cognitive functions.
LH Patients | ||||||||||||||||||||||
*From BADA ^ From BORE = education; loc = localization; F = frontal; i = insular; P = parietal; T = temporal; V = lesion volume (cc); OR = orientation; hans = handedness; STM = Short term memory; A buc = buccofacial apraxia; IMA = ideomotor apraxia; compreh = language comprehension; naming N = naming nouns; naming V = naming verbs; Phon flu = phonological fluency; W = words; PW = pseudowords; repe = repetition; Wr = writing; Phon D = phonological discrimination; A lex dec = auditory lexical decision. | ||||||||||||||||||||||
Case N# | Age | E | loc | V (cc) | O | R hand | Rave n | STM | A buc | IMA | Comp reh | Nami ng N | Nami ng V | Phon Fl | Read W | Read PW | Repe W | Repe PW | Wr W | Wr PW | Phon D | A Lex Dec |
P1 | 44 | 13 | F i P | 91.9 | v | 100 | ne | 20 | 72 | 35.5 | 29* | 27 | 16 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | |
P2 | 56 | 13 | TF i | 105.9 | V | 83.3 | 33/36 | ne | 20 | 72 | 34 | 30* | 28 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | ne | ne | |
P3 | 63 | 5 | F i | 115.3 | v | 100 | 25/36 | 5 | 20 | 71 | 35 | 30* | 27 | 35 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | ne | ne |
P4 | 29 | 13 | F i | 143.1 | V | 100 | 36/36 | ne | 19 | 72 | 34 | 24* | 24 | 6 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 30/30 | 40/40 |
P5 | 54 | 18 | F i | 79.1 | V | 100 | 33/36 | ne | 13 | 72 | 34 | 30* | 24 | 34 | 42/45 | 37/45 | 40/45 | 28/35 | 45/45 | 24/25 | 59/60 | 80/80 |
P6 | 56 | 13 | i | 104.9 | v | 100 | 33/36 | 6 | 20 | 72 | 35 | 30* | 26 | 27 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | ne | ne |
P7 | 49 | 8 | T i | 65.3 | v | 100 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 28 | 29* | 25 | 22 | 43/45 | 41/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 24/25 | ne | ne | |
P8 | 70 | 18 | FT i | 46.1 | v | 100 | 36/36 | ne | 20 | 72 | 35 | 29* | 26 | 19 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne |
P9 | 44 | 13 | T iF | 66 | v | 100 | 30/36 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 26 | 18* | 23 | 16 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne |
P10 | 44 | 13 | T i | 96.9 | v | 100 | 30/36 | 6 | 20 | 72 | 36 | 27* | 27 | 38 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne |
P11 | 38 | 13 | T i | 43.3 | v | 83.3 | 30/36 | 6 | 20 | 70 | 35 | 30* | 28 | 54 | 45/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 24/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 |
P12 | 33 | 18 | FT i | 28 | v | 50 | 34/36 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 36 | 30* | 28 | 53 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 |
P13 | 52 | 13 | FT i | 49.4 | v | 83, 3 | 24/36 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 35 | 26* | 27 | 15 | 45/45 | 43/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 44/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 79/80 |
P14 | 37 | 18 | f t i | 130 | v | 100 | 34/36 | 6 | 20 | 70 | 34 | 29* | 23 | 21 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 74/80 |
P15 | 49 | 13 | 228.3 | v | 100 | 29/36 | 6 | 20 | 63 | 28 | 27* | 21 | 11 | 43/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 30/30 | 72/80 | |
P16 | 32 | 13 | T i | 115.1 | v | 100 | 6 | 20 | 72 | 36 | 30* | 28 | 28 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 44/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 78/80 | |
P17 | 62 | 10 | T i | 154.8 | v | 100 | 29/36 | 6 | 20 | 66 | 32 | 26* | 23 | 18 | 45/45 | 43/45 | 45/45 | 34/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 63/80 |
P18 | 66 | 8 | i | 14.3 | v | 100 | ne | 4 | 20 | 72 | 35 | L 15/15 nl 17/20 ^ | 11 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | |
P19 | 45 | 13 | i | 196.4 | v | 41 | ne | 5 | 20 | 72 | 36 | L 12/15 nl 17/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P20 | 67 | 13 | T P i | 166.1 | v | 100 | ne | 5 | 20 | 72 | 31 | L 10/15 nl 16/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P21 | 44 | 13 | F T ins | 156 | v | 100 | ne | 6 | 20 | 72 | 36 | L 15/15 nl 20/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P22 | 44 | 8 | ins | 66.9 | v | 33 | ne | 5 | 20 | 72 | 36 | L 15/15 nl 20/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
Cutoff | - | - | - | 99 | - | 18 | 4 | 16 | 53 | 29 | 28*; L 8 nl 17 ^ | 26 | 17 | 43 | 43 | 45 | 35 | 43 | 23 | 58 | 78 |
Case N# | Age | E | loc | OR | hand | Raven | corsi | Costr A | Clock | bit star | TMT A | TMT B |
E = education; loc = lesion localization; F = frontal; i = insula; T = temporal; OR = orientation; Corsi = Spatial short term memory; Costr A = constructional apraxia; bit = behavioural attention test; TMT = trial making test. | ||||||||||||
P1 | 64 | 5 | FT i | v | 100 | n.e. | n.e. | 12 | 9 | n.e. | 38, 0e | 146, 1e |
P2 | 35 | 8 | i T | v | 100 | 22/36 | n.e. | 11 | 8 | 52 | ||
P3 | 69 | 5 | i | v | 83.33 | n.e. | 4 | 11 | n.e. | 52 | 25, 0e | 57, 0e |
P4 | 30 | 18 | FT i | v | 100 | 34/36 | n.e. | 14 | n.e. | 53 | ||
P5 | 47 | 13 | T i | v | 100 | n.e. | 14 | n.e. | 54 | 18, 0e | 53, 0e | |
P6 | 42 | 18 | FT i | v | 100 | 36/36 | 5 | 14 | 10 | 54 | ||
P7 | 37 | 13 | FP i | v | 100 | 30/36 | n.e. | 14 | n.e. | 54 | 39, 0e | 95, 0e |
P8 | 62 | 13 | T i | v | 100 | 34/36 | 5 | 14 | 9.5 | 54 | 26, 0e | 96, 1e |
P9 | 23 | 8 | T i | v | 100 | n.e. | 5 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 26, 0e | 104, 2e |
P10 | 43 | 13 | F i | v | 100 | 33/36 | n.e. | 14 | 9, 5 | 53 | 167, 0e | |
P11 | 71 | 13 | FT i | v | 100 | 34/36 | 5 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 25, 0e | 101, 0e |
P12 | 33 | 17 | i | v | 100 | 33/36 | 6 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 30, 0e | 70, 0e |
P13 | 35 | 13 | i | v | 100 | n.e. | 5 | 14 | n.e. | 16* | 30, 0e | 138, 0e |
P14 | 34 | 13 | FT i | v | 100 | n.e. | 4 | 14 | n.e. | 16* | n.e. | n.e. |
P15 | 35 | 18 | i FT | v | 60 | n.e. | 6 | 13 | n.e. | 22* | n.e. | n.e. |
P16 | 54 | 8 | T i | v | 100 | n.e. | 4 | 14 | n.e. | 22* | n.e. | n.e. |
P17 | 59 | 8 | i | v | 50 | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | 20* | n.e. | n.e. |
P18 | 37 | 13 | i | v | 50 | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | 20* | n.e. | n.e. |
Cutoff | - | - | - | 99 | - | 18 | 3.75 | 8 | 8.8 | 51; * balloon test< 16 | > 94 | > 283 |
Out of our 40 patients, some (N = 9 LH patients and N = 8 RH patients) also performed a follow up examination. In particular, we found that 44.44% of the LH patients who performed the follow up examination showed a comprehension deficit (Token test), 66.66% showed a noun naming deficit, 77.77% showed a verb naming deficit, 55.55% showed a phonological fluency impairment, 66.66% showed a word and pseudoword reading deficit. Other showed a repetition impairment (P1, P6 and P13), a writing deficit (P1 and P13) and a phonological discrimination deficit (P1). All the other tests were within the normal range (Table 4). To date these impaired patients (P2, P4, P6, P7 and P13) had lesions involving as shown by Figure 3 other regions in addition to the anterior insula (e.g, inferior frontal gyrus, premotor cortex; superior temporal gyrus), thus a direct causal relation between the deficit and the presence of a lesion involving the anterior insula cannot be proved unless a VLSM analysis is done; our sample size is too limited for performing such analysis. By contrast, the RH patients, with the exception of two of them who performed poorly on the visuo-spatial planning test (Clock test) (P2 and P12), performed within the normal range on all the tests (Table 4).
LH Patients Follow Up examination | |||||||||||||||||
Case N# | OR | STM | A buc | IMA | Compreh | Naming N | Naming V | Phon Flu | Read W | Read PW | Repe W | Rept PW | Wr W | Wr PW | Phon D | A Lex Dec | |
P2 | v | 5 | 16 | 55 | 20 | 17 | 20 | 15 | 34/45 | 28/35 | 43/45 | 27/35 | 7/15 | 20/25 | 54/60 | ne | |
P3 | V | ne | 20 | 72 | 30 | 5 | 6 | 8 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P4 | v | 5 | 20 | 71 | 26 | 24 | 26 | 10 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
P6 | V | ne | 20 | 72 | 18 | 6 | 5 | 8 | 35/45 | 30/45 | 45/45 | 30/35 | ne | ne | ne | ne | |
P7 | V | 5 | 20 | 72 | 23 | 26 | 23 | 20 | 43/45 | 41/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 24/25 | ne | ne | |
P11 | v | 6 | 20 | 70 | 35 | 30 | 28 | 45 | 45/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
P12 | v | 7 | 20 | 71 | 36 | 29 | 28 | 37 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
P13 | v | 5 | 13 | 62 | 32 | 27 | 22 | 12 | 88/92 | 24/45 | 45/45 | 34/35 | 35/45 | 22/25 | 60/60 | 79/80 | |
P16 | v | 6 | 20 | 72 | 35 | 28 | 25 | 34 | 44/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 44/45 | 24/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
Cut-off | - | 4 | 16 | 53 | 29 | 28 | 26 | 17 | 43 | 43 | 45 | 35 | 43 | 23 | 58 | 78 | |
RH Patients Follow Up examination | |||||||||||||||||
Case N# | OR | STM Spatial | Constructional Apraxia | Clock test | bit STAR | TMT A | TMT B | ||||||||||
P6 | v | ne | 11 | 9, 5 | 52 | 25, 0e | 51, 0e | ||||||||||
P7 | v | 5 | 14 | 0 | 54 | 33, 0e | 106, 1e | ||||||||||
P8 | v | ne | 12 | 10 | 34 | 70, 0e | 111, 0e | ||||||||||
P9 | v | 6 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 31, 0e | 108, 1e | ||||||||||
P10 | v | 4 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 30, 0e | 76, 0e | ||||||||||
P12 | v | 5 | 14 | 6.5 | 54 | 25, 0e | 80, 0e | ||||||||||
P13 | v | 4 | 14 | ne | 53 | ne | ne | ||||||||||
P14 | v | 4 | 14 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||||||||||
Cut-off | - | 3.75 | 8 | 8.8 | 51 | > 94 | > 283 |
We investigated the cognitive effects of neurosurgical lesions involving the insular cortex in a series of 22 patients with lesions involving the LH and 18 patients with lesions involving the RH. Different cognitive tests were administered to LH and RH patients. The involvement of the insula was ensured by performing a lesion overlapping analysis. The patients’ lesions were manually traced, normalized, and overlapped on a standard template. The maximum overlap of lesions involved the insula. In particular, the lesions involved the anterior insula as indicated by the overlapping of the probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps of the posterior insula obtained using the anatomy toolbox [40]. We created a ROI and we overlapped it (in violet) on the overlay plot of all the LH patients and that of RH patients. The lesions were anterior to the ROI comprising the posterior part of the insula. It is known that lesions involving the insula typically include and spread also to other surrounding areas and that patients with isolated insular lesions are very rare [14]. Also in the present study, the patients we included had a lesion involving in addition to the insula also the surrounding areas, however the lesion overlapping analysis showed that the maximum overlapping lesioned area involved the insula (mainly anterior) itself. Thus the interpretation of cognitive deficits in patients with insular neurosurgical lesions might be interpreted in the context of the complex interactions between the insula and its adjacent areas. Indeed the insula is located closely to the white matter pathways: the corticospinal tract runs in the superior posterior sector of the insula, the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus runs medially to the insula and the arcuate fasciculus runs more superiorly and could be very close to insular tumors [20]. Indeed the tendency of tumors to spread along the intricate network of afferent and efferent connections between the insula itself and surrounding cortical areas, constitute a major difficulty in reaching a gross total resection [20]. Thus, given that the insula is an integrative area linking information from different functional systems, i.e, cognitive, sensorimotor, social-emotional, and olfacto-gustatory [1,2,3], it is not surprising that the insular cortex is considered a multimodal processing area [3].
We found that pre-operatory symptoms significantly differed between patients with left and right insular lesions. In particular, a significant difference between cognitive and motor symptoms was observed, particularly in the case of LH patients, relative to RH patients. All the other categories of symptoms did not differ significantly between LH and RH patients.
The 38.88 of the RH patients and 18.18% of the LH patients reported anxiety/fear/panic attacks. This result is consistent with previous studies showing that patients with right insular lesions had a greater frequency of depressive and other mood disorders [39]. These effects might be related to disconnection between the insula and the frontal lobe or the anterior cingulate cortex, structures that have been associated with willed action and motor behaviour. The insula is an important neural substrate for representing emotional markers derived from internal states and through which perception of physiological responses may give raise to a conscious feedback on the internal state that guide behaviour [52]. In the model proposed by Craigh [53] the insula integrates the information about changes in bodily and cognitive states to create unitary feelings that encapsulate the “emotional now” or the “emotional self” [33]. Grey matter volume reduction of the left insula has been related to major depression [54,55] and bilateral reductions have been reported in posttraumatic stress disorder [56]. Similarly increased blow flow and metabolism have been reported in the insula of patients with major depression [57] and social anxiety disorders [58]. It has been proposed that patients with anxiety or depressive disorders interpret interoceptive states as threatening or negative states, which in turn cause hyper activity of the insula [59]. We add that RH more than LH patients seem to show anxiety/fear/panic attacks as pre-surgical symptoms.
We found that 27.77% of the RH patients and 4.5% of the LH patients reported taste sensations (e.g, bitter taste, strange gustatory sensations) in addition to 11.11% of the RH patients and none of the LH patients reported olfactory sensations (e.g, ointment odour). This is in line with fMRI results showing an insular activation in olfactory and gustatory tasks [60,61,62] and with intraoperative direct stimulation of the anterior insula studies reporting gustatory and olfactory sensations [16]. In particular, stimulation of the anterior part elicited “bad taste” or feelings of fear and stimulation of the posterior part elicited sensation in the mouth, salivation, increased gastric motility. Similarly our patients reported “bad taste” sensations as pre-operative symptoms. Patients’ studies have shown that lesions to insula might cause deficits in gustatory perception [14,63] or olfactory functions [34,64]. Insular epilepsy can trigger gustatory hallucinations [19]. Lesions to the right insula cause ipsilateral perception and recognition deficits, whereas lesion to the left insula cause ipsilateral deficit in taste perception and bilateral deficit in taste recognition [14,63]. Damage to the left insula can cause deficit in assigning taste adjectives to smell in the absence of olfactory processing impairments [34]. None of our RH patients showed olfactory symptoms, whereas there was a preponderance of RH patients, compared to LH patients, reporting gustatory sensations.
The 22.22% of the RH patients and 9% of the LH patients reported gastric sensations (e.g, stomach/nausea/cold sensations to the stomach). This result is consistent with stimulation studies reporting gastric sensations following stimulation of the insular cortex [17] and changes in gastrics motility [16]. Similarly a PET study [18] comparing patients with medial temporal lobe epilepsy who experienced visceral or emotional symptoms and those who did not, reported that visceral symptoms were correlated with hypometabolism of the posterior insula. In addition an fMRI study have shown that the insular cortex is activated during oesophageal distension [65]. Again, we found that there was a preponderance of RH patients compared to LH patients who showed gastric sensations.
In addition the 11.11% of the RH patients and none of the LH patients reported auditory sensations (e.g, increased sensibility, confusion). For example other authors found hypoacusia in the contralateral ear following stimulation of the isular cortex [17], auditory agnosia [35,36] and auditory temporal processing deficits [25] following insular infarction. Our data further confirm the crucial role of the right insula in auditory processing due to its functional role itself or to its connections to the auditory cortex.
As mentioned above, there was a significant dissociation between RH and LH patients as to the cognitive and motor symptoms. The 22.22% of the RH patients reported paresthesia (e.g, leg, arm) and none reported motor symptoms whereas the 27.27% of the LH patients reported motor symptoms (e.g, seizures involving the arm or the leg, or mouth). These results are consistent with the role of the insula as a multisensory and motor association area which maintains multiple connections to sensory and motor-relevant areas. In humans the insula and the ventral opercular premotor area contain somatotopically organized motor maps [66]. Somesthetic response (described as a sensation in the contralateral or both superior limbs) has been reported after left insular cortex stimulation [67]. Lastly, RH and LH patients significantly differed for cognitive symptoms. The 16.66% of the RH patients reported cognitive changes (e.g, attention, confusion) whereas the 63.63% of the LH patients presented cognitive changes (e.g, phonologic paraphasia, speech arrest, anomia). This aspect will be discussed in the next section addressing patients’ neuropsychological profiles.
In a recent review on the clinical effects of insular damage [32] multiple deficits arising following insular lesions have been reported involving autonomic, perceptual, somatosensory, vestibular systems, pain processing, stimuli awareness, emotion, mood and willed action, language, and memory processing. Similarly, other authors [33] reviewed the effects of insular lesions by focusing on emotional, perceptual, sensorimotor, and body awareness disorders. We found a strict parallelism between the pattern emerged in the pre-surgery symptoms analysis, and the patients’ cognitive profile. In particular, we found that LH patients showed more cognitive deficits than RH patients. A recent meta-analysis study of neuroimaging results [3] showed that the anterior-dorsal insula is activated by cognitive tasks. Consistent with this view we found that the 31.81% of the LH patients showed a noun naming deficit, the 36.26% showed a verb naming deficit and the 27.27% showed an impaired performance at the phonological fluency test. All the other tests were within the normal range. By contrast, the RH patients, with the exception of one case showing an impaired performance at the visuo-spatial planning test were within the normal range in performing all the tests. Right insular lesions can cause neglect [37,38]. This result fits with the pre-surgery symptoms analysis where the majority of the RH patients did not report changes or symptoms involving the cognitive functions. This result adds also a further indication about the role of the left insula cortex in language processing. We found language-related deficits in left insular patients, due to its functional role itself or to its connections to the language areas. Our results are not in line with previous neurosurgical studies reporting that the left insula can be removed without inducing definitive aphasia due to a preoperative language reshaping shown by fMRI [21]. Similarly, in another study of 42 neurosurgical patients with low-grade insular glioma, it has been shown that immediately following resection 10 patients experienced articulatory disorders while three moths post surgery there was no evidence of language disorders [22].
In addition there are other studies showing that deficit in sound detection and auditory temporal processing have been described in insula patients [25] but the lesions extended to adjacent areas involved in auditory processing. Broca’s aphasia [26] and apraxia of speech [27] have been related to insula lesions as well as repetition deficit [28]. A voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study showed that the verbal fluency was the most affected task by lesions in the insula [29]. Similarly our patients were pathological at the verbal fluency test. A meta-analysis of expressive language tasks showed that the left insula was involved among other areas in the network supporting speech production [30]. Another recent meta-analysis shown that receptive language, expressive language and speech production activate very similar regions of the anterior insula while activation related to perception of speech activates the left dorsal mid-insula [31]. Taken together our results suggest that language disorders can be reported in patients with left insula lesions and are consistent with a previous retrospective study of a series of 66 cases (19 right insula and 47 left insula) of surgery of insular nonenhancing gliomas [20] reporting that language disorders were found in the immediate postoperative phase in 11 patients (articulatory disorders in 1 patient, phonemic paraphasia without comprehension deficit in 8 patients, speech disorders with comprehension deficits in 2 patients). Our follow-up study evidenced that the examined patients, although being a small sample (N = 9 LH patients and N = 8 RH patients), still are pathological post-surgery. However, further studies are necessary to clarify whether selective lesions affecting the insula alone cause language related deficits or whether the pattern of results we found is rather due to the spreading of the lesions to surrounding areas (e.g, premotor cortex, temporal lobe). The insula has efferent and afferent connections to the temporal lobes. It has also been suggested that the critical areas in Broca’s region involved in speech production is the insula [24,68] whereas other authors argued that Broca’s area and not the insula is associated with articulatory impairments [69]. Indeed it has been shown that when lesions were restricted to the left insula [14] symptoms of aphasia were inconsistent. It is however unclear which language-related tasks have been administered. Thus the exact role of insula in language processing remains unclear. Interestingly, our data further indicate that the left insula might be involved in noun and verbs processing. Some authors [70] found that verb impairment emerged in four patients as a consequence of sub-cortical and insular damage in four of the patients belonging to a large sample of verb- and noun-impaired aphasic patients studied. In another study, the same group [71] showed that the left inferior frontal gyrus, the left insula, and the middle temporal gyrus are responsible for both noun and verb processing. A meta-analysis of action verbs processing found that the left insula was included in the network of areas consistently activated by action verbs [72].
We wish to thank the patients and the healthy volunteers and gratefully acknowledge the support by the MRI staff of the Unità Operativa di Neuroradiologia (A.O.S. Maria della Misericordia, Udine, Italy).
We disclose any potential sources of conflict of interest.
[1] |
Chen LM (2007) Imaging of pain. Int Anesthesiol Clin 45: 39-57. doi: 10.1097/AIA.0b013e31803419d3
![]() |
[2] | Craig AD (2002) How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 655-666. |
[3] |
Kurth F, Zilles K, Fox PT, et al. (2010) A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct 214: 519-534. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0255-z
![]() |
[4] | Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. (1982) Insula of the old world monkey. III: Efferent cortical output and comments on function. J Comp Neurol 212: 38-52. |
[5] | Mufson EJ, Mesulam MM. (1982) Insula of the old world monkey. II: Afferent cortical input and comments on the claustrum. J Comp Neurol 212: 23-37. |
[6] |
Nanetti L, Cerliani L, Gazzola V, et al. (2009) Group analyses of connectivity-based cortical parcellation using repeated k-means clustering. Neuroimage 47: 1666-1677. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.014
![]() |
[7] | Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Amin H, et al. (2010) Dissociable connectivity within human angular gyrus and intraparietal sulcus: evidence from functional and structural connectivity. Cereb Cortex20: 2636-2646. |
[8] | van den Heuvel MP, Hulshoff Pol HE. (2010) Specific somatotopic organization of functional connections of the primary motor network during resting state. Hum Brain Mapp 31: 631-644. |
[9] | Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. (1982) Insula of the old world monkey. III: Efferent cortical output and comments on function. J Comp Neurol 212: 38-52. |
[10] | Mufson EJ, Mesulam MM. (1982) Insula of the old world monkey. II: Afferent cortical input and comments on the claustrum. J Comp Neurol 212: 23-37. |
[11] |
Pritchard TC, Hamilton RB, Morse JR, et al. (1986) Projections of thalamic gustatory and lingual areas in the monkey, Macaca fascicularis. J Comp Neurol 244: 213-228. doi: 10.1002/cne.902440208
![]() |
[12] | Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. (1982) Insula of the old world monkey. III: Efferent cortical output and comments on function. J Comp Neurol 212: 38-52. |
[13] | Mufson EJ, Mesulam MM (1982) Insula of the old world monkey. II: Afferent cortical input and comments on the claustrum. J Comp Neurol 212: 23-37. |
[14] |
Cereda C, Ghika J, Maeder P, et al. (2002) Strokes restricted to the insular cortex. Neurology 59:1950-1955. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000038905.75660.BD
![]() |
[15] |
Fink JN, Selim MH, Kumar S, et al. (2005) Insular cortex infarction in acute middle cerebral artery territory stroke: predictor of stroke severity and vascular lesion. Arch Neurol 62:1081-1085. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.7.1081
![]() |
[16] |
Penfield W, Faulk ME. (1955) The insula; further observations on its function. Brain 78:445-470. doi: 10.1093/brain/78.4.445
![]() |
[17] |
Ostrowsky K, Isnard J, Ryvlin P, et al. (2000) Functional mapping of the insula cortex: clinical implication in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 41: 681-686. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.2000.tb00228.x
![]() |
[18] |
Dupont S, Bouilleret V, Hasboun D, et al. (2003) Functional anatomy of the insula: new insights from imaging. Surg Radiol Anat 25: 113-119. doi: 10.1007/s00276-003-0103-4
![]() |
[19] | Hausser-Hauw C, Bancaud J. (1987) Gustatory hallucinations in epileptic seizures. Electrophysiological, clinical and anatomical correlates. Brain 110 (Pt2): 339-359. |
[20] | Skrap M, Mondani M, Tomasino B, et al. (2012) Surgery of insular non-enhancing gliomas: volumetric analysis of tumoral resection, clinical outcome and survival in a consecutive series of 66 cases. Neurosurgery 70(5):1081-1093. |
[21] | Duffau H, Bauchet L, Lehericy S, et al. (2001) Functional compensation of the left dominant insula for language. Neuro Report 12: 2159-2163. |
[22] |
Duffau H, Taillandier L, Gatignol P, et al. (2006) The insular lobe and brain plasticity: Lessons from tumor surgery. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 108: 543-548. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2005.09.004
![]() |
[23] | McCarthy G, Gruetter R, Shulman RG, et al. (1993) Echo-planar magnetic resonance imaging studies of frontal cortex activation during word generation in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U. S. A90: 4952-4956. |
[24] | Price CJ. (2000) The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging. J Anat197: 335-359. |
[25] |
Bamiou DE, Musiek FE, Stow I, et al. (2006) Auditory temporal processing deficits in patients with insular stroke. Neurology 67: 614-619. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000230197.40410.db
![]() |
[26] |
Ardila A. (1999) The role of insula in language: an unsettled question. Aphasiology 13: 79-87. doi: 10.1080/026870399402334
![]() |
[27] |
Dronkers NF. (1996) A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature 384:159-161. doi: 10.1038/384159a0
![]() |
[28] | Carota A, Annoni JM, Marangolo P. (2007) Repeating through the insula: evidence from two consecutive strokes. Neuro Report 18: 1367-1370. |
[29] | Bates E, Wilson SM, Saygin AP, et al. (2003) Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Nature Neurosci 6: 448-450. |
[30] |
Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, et al. (2009) A systems perspective on the effective connectivity of overt speech production. Philos Trans A Math Phys Eng Sci 367: 2399-2421. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2008.0287
![]() |
[31] |
Oh A, Duerden EG, Pang EW. (2014) The role of the insula in speech and language processing. Brain Lang 135: 96-103. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.06.003
![]() |
[32] |
Ibanez A, Gleichgerrcht E, Manes F. (2010) Clinical effects of insular damage in humans. Brain Struct Funct 214: 397-410. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0256-y
![]() |
[33] |
Jones CL, Ward J, Critchley HD. (2010) The neuropsychological impact of insular cortex lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr 81: 611-618. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.193672
![]() |
[34] |
Stevenson RJ, Miller LA, Thayer ZC. (2008) Impairments in the perception of odor-induced tastes and their relationship to impairments in taste perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 34: 1183-1197. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1183
![]() |
[35] | Fifer RC. (1993) Insular stroke causing unilateral auditory processing disorder: case report. J Am Acad Audiol 4: 364-369. |
[36] |
Habib M, Daquin G, Milandre L. (1995) Mutism and auditory agnosia due to bilateral insular damage—role of the insula in human communication. Neuropsychologia 33: 327-339. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00108-2
![]() |
[37] | Karnath HO, Dieterich M. (2006) Spatial negelct—a vestibular disorder? Brain in Press. |
[38] |
Manes F, Paradiso S, Springer JA, et al. (1999) Neglect after right insular cortex infarction. Stroke 30: 946-948. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.30.5.946
![]() |
[39] |
Manes F, Paradiso S, Robinson RG. (1999) Neuropsychiatric effects of insular stroke. J Nerv Ment Dis 187: 707-712. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199912000-00001
![]() |
[40] |
Eickhoff S, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H. (2005) A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25: 1325-1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034
![]() |
[41] |
Oldfield RC. (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97-113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
![]() |
[42] | Basso A, Capitani E, Laiacona M, et al. (1987) Raven's coloured progressive matrices: normative values on 305 adult normal controls. Funct Neurol 2: 189-194. |
[43] | Orsini A, Grossi D, Capitani E, et al. (1987) Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: normative data from 1355 adults and 1112 children. Italian J Neurol Sci 8: 539-548. |
[44] | Spinnler M, Tognoni G. (1987) Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. Italian J Neurol Sci Suppl 8: 1-120. |
[45] | De Renzi E, Motti F, Nichelli P. (1980) Imitating gestures. A quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. Arch Neurol 37: 6-10. |
[46] |
De Renzi E, Faglioni P. (1978) Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the Token Test. Cortex 14: 41-49. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(78)80006-9
![]() |
[47] | Miceli G, Laudanna A, Burani C, et al. (1994) Batteria per l'analisi dei deficit afasici. B. A. D. A. In: B. A. D. A. A battery for the assessment of aphasic disorders. Roma: CEPSAG. |
[48] | Novelli G, Papagno C, Capitani E, et al. (1996) Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale. Taratura su soggetti normali. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr 47: 477-505. |
[49] | 50. Wilson BA, Cockburn J, Halligan PW. (1987) Behavioral inattention test (BIT). London: Thames Valley Test Company. |
[50] |
51. Giovagnoli AR, Del Pesce M, Mascheroni S, et al. (1996) Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Italian J Neurol Sci 17: 305-309. doi: 10.1007/BF01997792
![]() |
[51] | 52. Bechara A, Damasio A. (2014) The somatic marker hypothesis: a neural theory of economic decision. Games Econ Behav 52: 336-372. |
[52] | 53. Craig AD. (2009) How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 59-70. |
[53] |
54. Takahashi T, Malhi GS, Wood SJ, et al (2010) Insular cortex volume in established bipolar affective disorder: a preliminary MRI study. Psychiatr Res 182: 187-190. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.01.006
![]() |
[54] |
55. Takahashi T, Yucel M, Lorenzetti, et al (2010) Volumetric MRI study of the insular cortex in individuals with current and past major depression. J Affect Disord 121: 231-238. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.003
![]() |
[55] |
56. Chen S, Li L, Xu B, et al. (2009) Insular cortex involvement in declarative memory deficits in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder. BMC Psychiatr 9: 39. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-9-39
![]() |
[56] |
57. Drevets WC. (2000) Neuroimaging studies of mood disorders. Biol Psychiatr 48: 813-829. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01020-9
![]() |
[57] |
58. Klumpp H, Post D, Angstadt M, et al (2013) Anterior cingulate cortex and insula response during indirect and direct processing of emotional faces in generalized social anxiety disorder. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 3: 7. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-3-7
![]() |
[58] |
59. Paulus MP, Stein MB. (2006) An insular view of anxiety. Biol Psychiatr 60: 383-387. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042
![]() |
[59] |
60. Kringelbach ML, de Araujo IE, Rolls ET. (2004) Taste-related activity in the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 21: 781-788. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.063
![]() |
[60] | 61. Poellinger A, Thomas R, Lio P, et al (2001) Activation and habituation in olfaction—an fMRI study. Neuroimage 13: 547-5 |
[61] |
62. Royet JP, Plailly J (2004) Lateralization of olfactory processes. Chem Senses 29: 731-745. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh067
![]() |
[62] |
63. Pritchard TC, Macaluso DA, Eslinger PJ, et al (1999) Taste perception in patients with insular cortex lesions. Behav Neurosci 113: 663-671. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.113.4.663
![]() |
[63] |
64. Mak YE, Simmons KB, Gitelman DR, et al (2005) Taste and olfactory intensity perception changes following left insular stroke. Behav. Neurosci 119: 1693-1700. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.6.1693
![]() |
[64] | 65. Binkofski F, Schnitzler A, Enck P, et al.(1998) Somatic and limbic cortex activation in esophageal distention: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Ann Neurol 44: 811-815. |
[65] | 66. Fink GR, Frackowiak RS, Pietrzyk U, et al (1997) Multiple nonprimary motor areas in the human cortex 2. J Neurophysiol 77: 2164-2174. |
[66] |
67. Ostrowsky K, Isnard J, Ryvlin P, et al. (2000) Functional mapping of the insular cortex: clinical implication in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 41: 681-686. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.2000.tb00228.x
![]() |
[67] |
68. Wise RJ, Greene J, Buchel C, et al. (1999) Brain regions involved in articulation. Lancet 353:1057-1061. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)07491-1
![]() |
[68] |
69. Hillis AE, Work M, Barker PB, et al. (2004) Re-examining the brain regions crucial for orchestrating speech articulation. Brain 127: 1479-1487. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh172
![]() |
[69] | 70. Crepaldi D, Berlingeri M, Paulesu E, et al. (2011) A place for nouns and a place for verbs? A critical review of neurocognitive data on grammatical-class effects. Brain Lang 116: 33-49. |
[70] | 71. Crepaldi D, Berlingheri M, Cattinelli I, et al. (2013) Clustering the Lexicon in the brain: a meta-analysis of the neurofunctional evidence on noun and verb processing. Frontiers Human Neurosci 7: 303. |
1. | Barbara Tomasino, Maria Nobile, Marta Re, Monica Bellina, Marco Garzitto, Filippo Arrigoni, Massimo Molteni, Franco Fabbro, Paolo Brambilla, The mental simulation of state/psychological verbs in the adolescent brain: An fMRI study, 2018, 123, 02782626, 34, 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.02.010 | |
2. | Frédérique Roy-Côté, Rayane Zahal, Johannes Frasnelli, Dang Khoa Nguyen, Olivier Boucher, Insula and Olfaction: A Literature Review and Case Report, 2021, 11, 2076-3425, 198, 10.3390/brainsci11020198 | |
3. | Daphné Citherlet, Olivier Boucher, Julie Tremblay, Manon Robert, Anne Gallagher, Alain Bouthillier, Franco Lepore, Dang Khoa Nguyen, Spatiotemporal dynamics of auditory information processing in the insular cortex: an intracranial EEG study using an oddball paradigm, 2020, 225, 1863-2653, 1537, 10.1007/s00429-020-02072-z | |
4. | Barbara Tomasino, Dario Marin, Tamara Ius, Miran Skrap, 2018, Chapter 31, 978-3-319-75467-3, 281, 10.1007/978-3-319-75468-0_31 | |
5. | Gianna Sepede, Francesco Gambi, Massimo Di Giannantonio, Insular Dysfunction in People at Risk for Psychotic Disorders, 2015, 2, 2373-7972, 66, 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.2.66 | |
6. | Jarrod Moss, Introduction to AIMS Neuroscience Special Issue “What Function Does the Anterior Insula Play in Human Cognition?”, 2015, 2, 2373-7972, 153, 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.3.153 | |
7. | Olivier Boucher, Daphné Citherlet, Benjamin Hébert-Seropian, Dang Khoa Nguyen, 2018, Chapter 26, 978-3-319-75467-3, 223, 10.1007/978-3-319-75468-0_26 | |
8. | Elisa Cargnelutti, Marta Maieron, Serena D'Agostini, Tamara Ius, Miran Skrap, Barbara Tomasino, Preoperative plasticity in the functional naming network of patients with left insular gliomas, 2023, 22131582, 103561, 10.1016/j.nicl.2023.103561 | |
9. | Daniel J. O’Hara, John Goodden, Ryan Mathew, Rebecca Chan, Paul Chumas, Recovery of major cognitive deficits following awake surgery for insular glioma: a case report, 2024, 38, 0268-8697, 236, 10.1080/02688697.2020.1825620 | |
10. | Elisa Cargnelutti, Marta Maieron, Serena D’Agostini, Tamara Ius, Miran Skrap, Barbara Tomasino, Exploring cognitive Landscapes: Longitudinal Dynamics of left insula gliomas using neuropsychological Inquiry, fMRI and intra-resection real time neuropsychological testing, 2024, 22131582, 103689, 10.1016/j.nicl.2024.103689 |
RH Patients | ||||||||||||
anxiety/fear/panic attacks (p.a.) | epigastric: stomach/nausea | Fainting sensation | sensation | paresthesia | olfactory | sweating sensation | gustatory | auditory | motor | cognitive | ||
P1 anxiety, fear, p.a. | P2 stomach nausea, | P1 | P2 leg | P18 strange sensation | P5 | P9 difficulty in recognizing taste | P17 confusion | P6 distraction | ||||
P2 anxiety | P3 stomach | P8 arm, face | P11 ointment odor | P10 bitter taste | P9 > sensibility to sounds | P11 confusion | ||||||
P4 p.a. | P7 epigastric | P12 arm, face | P15 gustatory hallucinations | P16 prosopoagnosia | ||||||||
P5 p.a, anxiety depression | P12 stomach | P15 left side of the body | P18 strange taste | |||||||||
P6 anxiety | P4 strange taste | |||||||||||
P12 pleasure and anxiety | ||||||||||||
P15 anxiety | ||||||||||||
LH patients | ||||||||||||
P1 p.a. | P6 senso di vomito | - | P10 hand, leg and head | - | - | P1 strong breaths from the nose as hot air | P16 bitter taste | - | P2 leg stiffening | P1 Phon par | ||
P10 anxiety | P9 intenso freddo allo stomaco | P16 right side of the body | P3 seizure arm and leg | P2 confusion | ||||||||
P11 p.a. | P18 hand, leg | P5 seizure arm and face | P3 speech arrest | |||||||||
P14 unpleasent | P22 hand | P13 face | P4 anomia | |||||||||
sensations, anxiety | and cheeck | |||||||||||
P18 hand stiffening | P5 diff to speak phon paraf | |||||||||||
P19 leg stiffening | P7 confusion | |||||||||||
P8 anomia | ||||||||||||
P13 diff speak | ||||||||||||
P14 anomia | ||||||||||||
P15 verbal paraphasia | ||||||||||||
P17 diff to speak | ||||||||||||
P18 articulation and anomia | ||||||||||||
P19 anomia and stm | ||||||||||||
P20anomia phon paraph |
LH Patients | ||||||||||||||||||||||
*From BADA ^ From BORE = education; loc = localization; F = frontal; i = insular; P = parietal; T = temporal; V = lesion volume (cc); OR = orientation; hans = handedness; STM = Short term memory; A buc = buccofacial apraxia; IMA = ideomotor apraxia; compreh = language comprehension; naming N = naming nouns; naming V = naming verbs; Phon flu = phonological fluency; W = words; PW = pseudowords; repe = repetition; Wr = writing; Phon D = phonological discrimination; A lex dec = auditory lexical decision. | ||||||||||||||||||||||
Case N# | Age | E | loc | V (cc) | O | R hand | Rave n | STM | A buc | IMA | Comp reh | Nami ng N | Nami ng V | Phon Fl | Read W | Read PW | Repe W | Repe PW | Wr W | Wr PW | Phon D | A Lex Dec |
P1 | 44 | 13 | F i P | 91.9 | v | 100 | ne | 20 | 72 | 35.5 | 29* | 27 | 16 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | |
P2 | 56 | 13 | TF i | 105.9 | V | 83.3 | 33/36 | ne | 20 | 72 | 34 | 30* | 28 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | ne | ne | |
P3 | 63 | 5 | F i | 115.3 | v | 100 | 25/36 | 5 | 20 | 71 | 35 | 30* | 27 | 35 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | ne | ne |
P4 | 29 | 13 | F i | 143.1 | V | 100 | 36/36 | ne | 19 | 72 | 34 | 24* | 24 | 6 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 30/30 | 40/40 |
P5 | 54 | 18 | F i | 79.1 | V | 100 | 33/36 | ne | 13 | 72 | 34 | 30* | 24 | 34 | 42/45 | 37/45 | 40/45 | 28/35 | 45/45 | 24/25 | 59/60 | 80/80 |
P6 | 56 | 13 | i | 104.9 | v | 100 | 33/36 | 6 | 20 | 72 | 35 | 30* | 26 | 27 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | ne | ne |
P7 | 49 | 8 | T i | 65.3 | v | 100 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 28 | 29* | 25 | 22 | 43/45 | 41/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 24/25 | ne | ne | |
P8 | 70 | 18 | FT i | 46.1 | v | 100 | 36/36 | ne | 20 | 72 | 35 | 29* | 26 | 19 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne |
P9 | 44 | 13 | T iF | 66 | v | 100 | 30/36 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 26 | 18* | 23 | 16 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne |
P10 | 44 | 13 | T i | 96.9 | v | 100 | 30/36 | 6 | 20 | 72 | 36 | 27* | 27 | 38 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne |
P11 | 38 | 13 | T i | 43.3 | v | 83.3 | 30/36 | 6 | 20 | 70 | 35 | 30* | 28 | 54 | 45/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 24/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 |
P12 | 33 | 18 | FT i | 28 | v | 50 | 34/36 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 36 | 30* | 28 | 53 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 |
P13 | 52 | 13 | FT i | 49.4 | v | 83, 3 | 24/36 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 35 | 26* | 27 | 15 | 45/45 | 43/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 44/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 79/80 |
P14 | 37 | 18 | f t i | 130 | v | 100 | 34/36 | 6 | 20 | 70 | 34 | 29* | 23 | 21 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 74/80 |
P15 | 49 | 13 | 228.3 | v | 100 | 29/36 | 6 | 20 | 63 | 28 | 27* | 21 | 11 | 43/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 30/30 | 72/80 | |
P16 | 32 | 13 | T i | 115.1 | v | 100 | 6 | 20 | 72 | 36 | 30* | 28 | 28 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 44/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 78/80 | |
P17 | 62 | 10 | T i | 154.8 | v | 100 | 29/36 | 6 | 20 | 66 | 32 | 26* | 23 | 18 | 45/45 | 43/45 | 45/45 | 34/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 63/80 |
P18 | 66 | 8 | i | 14.3 | v | 100 | ne | 4 | 20 | 72 | 35 | L 15/15 nl 17/20 ^ | 11 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | |
P19 | 45 | 13 | i | 196.4 | v | 41 | ne | 5 | 20 | 72 | 36 | L 12/15 nl 17/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P20 | 67 | 13 | T P i | 166.1 | v | 100 | ne | 5 | 20 | 72 | 31 | L 10/15 nl 16/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P21 | 44 | 13 | F T ins | 156 | v | 100 | ne | 6 | 20 | 72 | 36 | L 15/15 nl 20/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P22 | 44 | 8 | ins | 66.9 | v | 33 | ne | 5 | 20 | 72 | 36 | L 15/15 nl 20/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
Cutoff | - | - | - | 99 | - | 18 | 4 | 16 | 53 | 29 | 28*; L 8 nl 17 ^ | 26 | 17 | 43 | 43 | 45 | 35 | 43 | 23 | 58 | 78 |
Case N# | Age | E | loc | OR | hand | Raven | corsi | Costr A | Clock | bit star | TMT A | TMT B |
E = education; loc = lesion localization; F = frontal; i = insula; T = temporal; OR = orientation; Corsi = Spatial short term memory; Costr A = constructional apraxia; bit = behavioural attention test; TMT = trial making test. | ||||||||||||
P1 | 64 | 5 | FT i | v | 100 | n.e. | n.e. | 12 | 9 | n.e. | 38, 0e | 146, 1e |
P2 | 35 | 8 | i T | v | 100 | 22/36 | n.e. | 11 | 8 | 52 | ||
P3 | 69 | 5 | i | v | 83.33 | n.e. | 4 | 11 | n.e. | 52 | 25, 0e | 57, 0e |
P4 | 30 | 18 | FT i | v | 100 | 34/36 | n.e. | 14 | n.e. | 53 | ||
P5 | 47 | 13 | T i | v | 100 | n.e. | 14 | n.e. | 54 | 18, 0e | 53, 0e | |
P6 | 42 | 18 | FT i | v | 100 | 36/36 | 5 | 14 | 10 | 54 | ||
P7 | 37 | 13 | FP i | v | 100 | 30/36 | n.e. | 14 | n.e. | 54 | 39, 0e | 95, 0e |
P8 | 62 | 13 | T i | v | 100 | 34/36 | 5 | 14 | 9.5 | 54 | 26, 0e | 96, 1e |
P9 | 23 | 8 | T i | v | 100 | n.e. | 5 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 26, 0e | 104, 2e |
P10 | 43 | 13 | F i | v | 100 | 33/36 | n.e. | 14 | 9, 5 | 53 | 167, 0e | |
P11 | 71 | 13 | FT i | v | 100 | 34/36 | 5 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 25, 0e | 101, 0e |
P12 | 33 | 17 | i | v | 100 | 33/36 | 6 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 30, 0e | 70, 0e |
P13 | 35 | 13 | i | v | 100 | n.e. | 5 | 14 | n.e. | 16* | 30, 0e | 138, 0e |
P14 | 34 | 13 | FT i | v | 100 | n.e. | 4 | 14 | n.e. | 16* | n.e. | n.e. |
P15 | 35 | 18 | i FT | v | 60 | n.e. | 6 | 13 | n.e. | 22* | n.e. | n.e. |
P16 | 54 | 8 | T i | v | 100 | n.e. | 4 | 14 | n.e. | 22* | n.e. | n.e. |
P17 | 59 | 8 | i | v | 50 | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | 20* | n.e. | n.e. |
P18 | 37 | 13 | i | v | 50 | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | 20* | n.e. | n.e. |
Cutoff | - | - | - | 99 | - | 18 | 3.75 | 8 | 8.8 | 51; * balloon test< 16 | > 94 | > 283 |
LH Patients Follow Up examination | |||||||||||||||||
Case N# | OR | STM | A buc | IMA | Compreh | Naming N | Naming V | Phon Flu | Read W | Read PW | Repe W | Rept PW | Wr W | Wr PW | Phon D | A Lex Dec | |
P2 | v | 5 | 16 | 55 | 20 | 17 | 20 | 15 | 34/45 | 28/35 | 43/45 | 27/35 | 7/15 | 20/25 | 54/60 | ne | |
P3 | V | ne | 20 | 72 | 30 | 5 | 6 | 8 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P4 | v | 5 | 20 | 71 | 26 | 24 | 26 | 10 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
P6 | V | ne | 20 | 72 | 18 | 6 | 5 | 8 | 35/45 | 30/45 | 45/45 | 30/35 | ne | ne | ne | ne | |
P7 | V | 5 | 20 | 72 | 23 | 26 | 23 | 20 | 43/45 | 41/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 24/25 | ne | ne | |
P11 | v | 6 | 20 | 70 | 35 | 30 | 28 | 45 | 45/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
P12 | v | 7 | 20 | 71 | 36 | 29 | 28 | 37 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
P13 | v | 5 | 13 | 62 | 32 | 27 | 22 | 12 | 88/92 | 24/45 | 45/45 | 34/35 | 35/45 | 22/25 | 60/60 | 79/80 | |
P16 | v | 6 | 20 | 72 | 35 | 28 | 25 | 34 | 44/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 44/45 | 24/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
Cut-off | - | 4 | 16 | 53 | 29 | 28 | 26 | 17 | 43 | 43 | 45 | 35 | 43 | 23 | 58 | 78 | |
RH Patients Follow Up examination | |||||||||||||||||
Case N# | OR | STM Spatial | Constructional Apraxia | Clock test | bit STAR | TMT A | TMT B | ||||||||||
P6 | v | ne | 11 | 9, 5 | 52 | 25, 0e | 51, 0e | ||||||||||
P7 | v | 5 | 14 | 0 | 54 | 33, 0e | 106, 1e | ||||||||||
P8 | v | ne | 12 | 10 | 34 | 70, 0e | 111, 0e | ||||||||||
P9 | v | 6 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 31, 0e | 108, 1e | ||||||||||
P10 | v | 4 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 30, 0e | 76, 0e | ||||||||||
P12 | v | 5 | 14 | 6.5 | 54 | 25, 0e | 80, 0e | ||||||||||
P13 | v | 4 | 14 | ne | 53 | ne | ne | ||||||||||
P14 | v | 4 | 14 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||||||||||
Cut-off | - | 3.75 | 8 | 8.8 | 51 | > 94 | > 283 |
RH Patients | ||||||||||||
anxiety/fear/panic attacks (p.a.) | epigastric: stomach/nausea | Fainting sensation | sensation | paresthesia | olfactory | sweating sensation | gustatory | auditory | motor | cognitive | ||
P1 anxiety, fear, p.a. | P2 stomach nausea, | P1 | P2 leg | P18 strange sensation | P5 | P9 difficulty in recognizing taste | P17 confusion | P6 distraction | ||||
P2 anxiety | P3 stomach | P8 arm, face | P11 ointment odor | P10 bitter taste | P9 > sensibility to sounds | P11 confusion | ||||||
P4 p.a. | P7 epigastric | P12 arm, face | P15 gustatory hallucinations | P16 prosopoagnosia | ||||||||
P5 p.a, anxiety depression | P12 stomach | P15 left side of the body | P18 strange taste | |||||||||
P6 anxiety | P4 strange taste | |||||||||||
P12 pleasure and anxiety | ||||||||||||
P15 anxiety | ||||||||||||
LH patients | ||||||||||||
P1 p.a. | P6 senso di vomito | - | P10 hand, leg and head | - | - | P1 strong breaths from the nose as hot air | P16 bitter taste | - | P2 leg stiffening | P1 Phon par | ||
P10 anxiety | P9 intenso freddo allo stomaco | P16 right side of the body | P3 seizure arm and leg | P2 confusion | ||||||||
P11 p.a. | P18 hand, leg | P5 seizure arm and face | P3 speech arrest | |||||||||
P14 unpleasent | P22 hand | P13 face | P4 anomia | |||||||||
sensations, anxiety | and cheeck | |||||||||||
P18 hand stiffening | P5 diff to speak phon paraf | |||||||||||
P19 leg stiffening | P7 confusion | |||||||||||
P8 anomia | ||||||||||||
P13 diff speak | ||||||||||||
P14 anomia | ||||||||||||
P15 verbal paraphasia | ||||||||||||
P17 diff to speak | ||||||||||||
P18 articulation and anomia | ||||||||||||
P19 anomia and stm | ||||||||||||
P20anomia phon paraph |
LH Patients | ||||||||||||||||||||||
*From BADA ^ From BORE = education; loc = localization; F = frontal; i = insular; P = parietal; T = temporal; V = lesion volume (cc); OR = orientation; hans = handedness; STM = Short term memory; A buc = buccofacial apraxia; IMA = ideomotor apraxia; compreh = language comprehension; naming N = naming nouns; naming V = naming verbs; Phon flu = phonological fluency; W = words; PW = pseudowords; repe = repetition; Wr = writing; Phon D = phonological discrimination; A lex dec = auditory lexical decision. | ||||||||||||||||||||||
Case N# | Age | E | loc | V (cc) | O | R hand | Rave n | STM | A buc | IMA | Comp reh | Nami ng N | Nami ng V | Phon Fl | Read W | Read PW | Repe W | Repe PW | Wr W | Wr PW | Phon D | A Lex Dec |
P1 | 44 | 13 | F i P | 91.9 | v | 100 | ne | 20 | 72 | 35.5 | 29* | 27 | 16 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | |
P2 | 56 | 13 | TF i | 105.9 | V | 83.3 | 33/36 | ne | 20 | 72 | 34 | 30* | 28 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | ne | ne | |
P3 | 63 | 5 | F i | 115.3 | v | 100 | 25/36 | 5 | 20 | 71 | 35 | 30* | 27 | 35 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | ne | ne |
P4 | 29 | 13 | F i | 143.1 | V | 100 | 36/36 | ne | 19 | 72 | 34 | 24* | 24 | 6 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 30/30 | 40/40 |
P5 | 54 | 18 | F i | 79.1 | V | 100 | 33/36 | ne | 13 | 72 | 34 | 30* | 24 | 34 | 42/45 | 37/45 | 40/45 | 28/35 | 45/45 | 24/25 | 59/60 | 80/80 |
P6 | 56 | 13 | i | 104.9 | v | 100 | 33/36 | 6 | 20 | 72 | 35 | 30* | 26 | 27 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | ne | ne |
P7 | 49 | 8 | T i | 65.3 | v | 100 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 28 | 29* | 25 | 22 | 43/45 | 41/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 24/25 | ne | ne | |
P8 | 70 | 18 | FT i | 46.1 | v | 100 | 36/36 | ne | 20 | 72 | 35 | 29* | 26 | 19 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne |
P9 | 44 | 13 | T iF | 66 | v | 100 | 30/36 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 26 | 18* | 23 | 16 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne |
P10 | 44 | 13 | T i | 96.9 | v | 100 | 30/36 | 6 | 20 | 72 | 36 | 27* | 27 | 38 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne |
P11 | 38 | 13 | T i | 43.3 | v | 83.3 | 30/36 | 6 | 20 | 70 | 35 | 30* | 28 | 54 | 45/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 24/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 |
P12 | 33 | 18 | FT i | 28 | v | 50 | 34/36 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 36 | 30* | 28 | 53 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 |
P13 | 52 | 13 | FT i | 49.4 | v | 83, 3 | 24/36 | 5 | 20 | 72 | 35 | 26* | 27 | 15 | 45/45 | 43/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 44/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 79/80 |
P14 | 37 | 18 | f t i | 130 | v | 100 | 34/36 | 6 | 20 | 70 | 34 | 29* | 23 | 21 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 74/80 |
P15 | 49 | 13 | 228.3 | v | 100 | 29/36 | 6 | 20 | 63 | 28 | 27* | 21 | 11 | 43/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 30/30 | 72/80 | |
P16 | 32 | 13 | T i | 115.1 | v | 100 | 6 | 20 | 72 | 36 | 30* | 28 | 28 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 44/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 78/80 | |
P17 | 62 | 10 | T i | 154.8 | v | 100 | 29/36 | 6 | 20 | 66 | 32 | 26* | 23 | 18 | 45/45 | 43/45 | 45/45 | 34/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 63/80 |
P18 | 66 | 8 | i | 14.3 | v | 100 | ne | 4 | 20 | 72 | 35 | L 15/15 nl 17/20 ^ | 11 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | |
P19 | 45 | 13 | i | 196.4 | v | 41 | ne | 5 | 20 | 72 | 36 | L 12/15 nl 17/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P20 | 67 | 13 | T P i | 166.1 | v | 100 | ne | 5 | 20 | 72 | 31 | L 10/15 nl 16/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P21 | 44 | 13 | F T ins | 156 | v | 100 | ne | 6 | 20 | 72 | 36 | L 15/15 nl 20/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P22 | 44 | 8 | ins | 66.9 | v | 33 | ne | 5 | 20 | 72 | 36 | L 15/15 nl 20/20 ^ | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
Cutoff | - | - | - | 99 | - | 18 | 4 | 16 | 53 | 29 | 28*; L 8 nl 17 ^ | 26 | 17 | 43 | 43 | 45 | 35 | 43 | 23 | 58 | 78 |
Case N# | Age | E | loc | OR | hand | Raven | corsi | Costr A | Clock | bit star | TMT A | TMT B |
E = education; loc = lesion localization; F = frontal; i = insula; T = temporal; OR = orientation; Corsi = Spatial short term memory; Costr A = constructional apraxia; bit = behavioural attention test; TMT = trial making test. | ||||||||||||
P1 | 64 | 5 | FT i | v | 100 | n.e. | n.e. | 12 | 9 | n.e. | 38, 0e | 146, 1e |
P2 | 35 | 8 | i T | v | 100 | 22/36 | n.e. | 11 | 8 | 52 | ||
P3 | 69 | 5 | i | v | 83.33 | n.e. | 4 | 11 | n.e. | 52 | 25, 0e | 57, 0e |
P4 | 30 | 18 | FT i | v | 100 | 34/36 | n.e. | 14 | n.e. | 53 | ||
P5 | 47 | 13 | T i | v | 100 | n.e. | 14 | n.e. | 54 | 18, 0e | 53, 0e | |
P6 | 42 | 18 | FT i | v | 100 | 36/36 | 5 | 14 | 10 | 54 | ||
P7 | 37 | 13 | FP i | v | 100 | 30/36 | n.e. | 14 | n.e. | 54 | 39, 0e | 95, 0e |
P8 | 62 | 13 | T i | v | 100 | 34/36 | 5 | 14 | 9.5 | 54 | 26, 0e | 96, 1e |
P9 | 23 | 8 | T i | v | 100 | n.e. | 5 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 26, 0e | 104, 2e |
P10 | 43 | 13 | F i | v | 100 | 33/36 | n.e. | 14 | 9, 5 | 53 | 167, 0e | |
P11 | 71 | 13 | FT i | v | 100 | 34/36 | 5 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 25, 0e | 101, 0e |
P12 | 33 | 17 | i | v | 100 | 33/36 | 6 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 30, 0e | 70, 0e |
P13 | 35 | 13 | i | v | 100 | n.e. | 5 | 14 | n.e. | 16* | 30, 0e | 138, 0e |
P14 | 34 | 13 | FT i | v | 100 | n.e. | 4 | 14 | n.e. | 16* | n.e. | n.e. |
P15 | 35 | 18 | i FT | v | 60 | n.e. | 6 | 13 | n.e. | 22* | n.e. | n.e. |
P16 | 54 | 8 | T i | v | 100 | n.e. | 4 | 14 | n.e. | 22* | n.e. | n.e. |
P17 | 59 | 8 | i | v | 50 | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | 20* | n.e. | n.e. |
P18 | 37 | 13 | i | v | 50 | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | n.e. | 20* | n.e. | n.e. |
Cutoff | - | - | - | 99 | - | 18 | 3.75 | 8 | 8.8 | 51; * balloon test< 16 | > 94 | > 283 |
LH Patients Follow Up examination | |||||||||||||||||
Case N# | OR | STM | A buc | IMA | Compreh | Naming N | Naming V | Phon Flu | Read W | Read PW | Repe W | Rept PW | Wr W | Wr PW | Phon D | A Lex Dec | |
P2 | v | 5 | 16 | 55 | 20 | 17 | 20 | 15 | 34/45 | 28/35 | 43/45 | 27/35 | 7/15 | 20/25 | 54/60 | ne | |
P3 | V | ne | 20 | 72 | 30 | 5 | 6 | 8 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||
P4 | v | 5 | 20 | 71 | 26 | 24 | 26 | 10 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
P6 | V | ne | 20 | 72 | 18 | 6 | 5 | 8 | 35/45 | 30/45 | 45/45 | 30/35 | ne | ne | ne | ne | |
P7 | V | 5 | 20 | 72 | 23 | 26 | 23 | 20 | 43/45 | 41/45 | 30/30 | 30/30 | 45/45 | 24/25 | ne | ne | |
P11 | v | 6 | 20 | 70 | 35 | 30 | 28 | 45 | 45/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
P12 | v | 7 | 20 | 71 | 36 | 29 | 28 | 37 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 45/45 | 25/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
P13 | v | 5 | 13 | 62 | 32 | 27 | 22 | 12 | 88/92 | 24/45 | 45/45 | 34/35 | 35/45 | 22/25 | 60/60 | 79/80 | |
P16 | v | 6 | 20 | 72 | 35 | 28 | 25 | 34 | 44/45 | 44/45 | 45/45 | 35/35 | 44/45 | 24/25 | 60/60 | 80/80 | |
Cut-off | - | 4 | 16 | 53 | 29 | 28 | 26 | 17 | 43 | 43 | 45 | 35 | 43 | 23 | 58 | 78 | |
RH Patients Follow Up examination | |||||||||||||||||
Case N# | OR | STM Spatial | Constructional Apraxia | Clock test | bit STAR | TMT A | TMT B | ||||||||||
P6 | v | ne | 11 | 9, 5 | 52 | 25, 0e | 51, 0e | ||||||||||
P7 | v | 5 | 14 | 0 | 54 | 33, 0e | 106, 1e | ||||||||||
P8 | v | ne | 12 | 10 | 34 | 70, 0e | 111, 0e | ||||||||||
P9 | v | 6 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 31, 0e | 108, 1e | ||||||||||
P10 | v | 4 | 14 | 10 | 54 | 30, 0e | 76, 0e | ||||||||||
P12 | v | 5 | 14 | 6.5 | 54 | 25, 0e | 80, 0e | ||||||||||
P13 | v | 4 | 14 | ne | 53 | ne | ne | ||||||||||
P14 | v | 4 | 14 | ne | ne | ne | ne | ||||||||||
Cut-off | - | 3.75 | 8 | 8.8 | 51 | > 94 | > 283 |