Commentary Recurring Topics

Phasic Sleep Events Shape Cognitive Function after Traumatic Brain Injury: Implications for the Study of Sleep in Neurodevelopmental Disorders

  • The biological functions of sleep have long eluded the medical and research community. In four consecutive issues of AIMS Neuroscience, original and review manuscripts were recently published regarding the mechanisms and function of sleep. These articles highlight the well-timed topic of quantitative sleep markers and cognitive functioning as one of extensive interest within the field of neuroscience. Our commentary on the original research performed by Cote, Milner, and Speth (2015) brings attention to the importance of examining individual differences in sleep and cognition in subjects with traumatic brain injury (TBI), and provides support for conducting similar sleep analyses in neurodevelopmental disorders.

    Citation: Carolyn E. Jones, Miranda M. Lim. Phasic Sleep Events Shape Cognitive Function after Traumatic Brain Injury: Implications for the Study of Sleep in Neurodevelopmental Disorders[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(2): 232-236. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.2.232

    Related Papers:

    [1] Kimberly A Cote, Catherine E Milner, Tamara A Speth . Altered Sleep Mechanisms following Traumatic Brain Injury and Relation to Waking Function. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(4): 203-228. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.4.203
    [2] Mark R. Zielinski, James T. McKenna, Robert W. McCarley . Functions and Mechanisms of Sleep. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(1): 67-104. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.1.67
    [3] Daniel A. Barone, Ana C. Krieger . The Function of Sleep. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(2): 71-90. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.2.71
    [4] Marie Vandekerckhove, Yu-lin Wang . Emotion, emotion regulation and sleep: An intimate relationship. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(1): 1-17. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.1.1
    [5] Monireh Asadi Ghaleni, Forouzan Fattahi Masrour, Narjes Saryar, Alexandra J. Bratty, Ebrahim Norouzi, Matheus Santos de Sousa Fernandes, Georgian Badicu . Effects of an intervention combining physical activity and components of Amygdala and Insula Retraining (AIR) on sleep and working memory among older male adults. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(4): 421-438. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024025
    [6] Samson Z Assefa, Montserrat Diaz-Abad, Emerson M Wickwire, Steven M Scharf . The Functions of Sleep. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(3): 155-171. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.3.155
    [7] Zarina Greenberg, Hayley Ramshaw, Quenten Schwarz . Time Windows of Interneuron Development: Implications to Our Understanding of the Aetiology and Treatment of Schizophrenia. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(4): 294-321. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.4.294
    [8] Andrea J. Rapkin, Steven M. Berman, Edythe D. London . The Cerebellum and Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder. AIMS Neuroscience, 2014, 1(2): 120-141. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.2.120
    [9] Mohamad Arif Awang Nawi, Nor Farid Mohd Noor, Ramizu Shaari, Ameera Kamal Khaleel, Muhamamd Amirul Mat Lazin, Ibrahim Mohammed Sulaiman, Mustafa Mamat . The patterns of facial fractures in traumatic brain injury (TBI) patients using ordinal regression: a retrospective study of five years. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(3): 345-357. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022019
    [10] Alexis M. Chambers, Jessica D. Payne . The Influence of Sleep on the Consolidation of Positive Emotional Memories: Preliminary Evidence. AIMS Neuroscience, 2014, 1(1): 39-51. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.1.39
  • The biological functions of sleep have long eluded the medical and research community. In four consecutive issues of AIMS Neuroscience, original and review manuscripts were recently published regarding the mechanisms and function of sleep. These articles highlight the well-timed topic of quantitative sleep markers and cognitive functioning as one of extensive interest within the field of neuroscience. Our commentary on the original research performed by Cote, Milner, and Speth (2015) brings attention to the importance of examining individual differences in sleep and cognition in subjects with traumatic brain injury (TBI), and provides support for conducting similar sleep analyses in neurodevelopmental disorders.


    1. The Elusive Function of Sleep

    We read with interest the past 4 issues of AIMS Neuroscience, where the timely topic of the biological function of sleep was reviewed over multiple issues (see [1,2,3]). From these reviews, it is evident that despite extensive knowledge of the cellular mechanisms and the neuroanatomical underpinnings of sleep, the function of sleep remains elusive, with the majority of research conclusions derived from observations of the immediate effects of acute or chronic sleep deprivation.

    In their review, Zielinski, McKenna, and McCarley (2016) provide a comprehensive review on the underlying physiology of sleep regulation along with the cellular and molecular pathways activated or inhibited by sleep and wake. Barone and Krieger (2015) discuss the potential ecological purposes of sleep and provide an in-depth review of how sleep impacts overall health. Assefa and colleagues (2015) provide a concise history of sleep research and summarize what we know about the biological functions of sleep by reviewing mostly neurocognitive outcomes associated with sleep deprivation. Although each of these articles provides an accurate summary or the available data regarding sleep structure and function in adults, their back-to-back publications in AIMS Neuroscience stresses the challenges the field faces in providing answers regarding the function of sleep. Notably, none of these articles reviewed the literature on the function of sleep in early development.


    2. Properties of Phasic Sleep Events Predict Cognitive Ability after TBI—Commentary on Cote, Milner, and Speth (2015)

    Within these recent issues, highly relevant original research by Cote, Milner, and Speth (2015) highlighted the importance of individual differences when investigating sleep function in patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI). In a research article entitled “Altered Sleep Mechanisms following Traumatic Brain Injury and Relation to Waking Function”, Cote and colleagues investigated neurophysiological features of sleep in patients with traumatic brain injury using multiple night assessments (subjects spent three nights in testing). Importantly, the authors took into account TBI severity (as determined by length of unconsciousness after injury) when conducting analyses in order to parse out the influence of injury from specific characteristics of their sleep structure on waking cognitive functioning.

    Consistent with other work [4] (for review see: [5]), Cote et al. found that patients with TBI demonstrated disrupted sleep. Specifically, patients with TBI reported longer times to fall asleep and less time spent sleeping. The polysomnograms confirmed increased latency to sleep times but indicated no difference in total sleep time when compared with healthy controls [6]. However, patients with TBI had significantly decreased delta power at all sleep stages compared with controls, which likely accounted for the subjective feeling of poor overall sleep. The research presented by Cote and colleagues (2015) explicitly explored properties of phasic sleep events by analyzing both spontaneous and stimulus evoked k-complexes and spindles, thought to reflect inhibition of information processing during sleep. Amongst many other findings, they showed that TBI patients went longer between spontaneous k-complexes (lower density) than controls and had fewer evoked k-complexes in response to stimulus presentation [6].

    Within subjects, there was between night variability in multiple sleep domains (e.g. sigma power, spindle duration) that was only present in the TBI patient group. Other researchers have also noted intra-subject variability in sleep patterns across nights in patient populations (e.g. the elderly [7], children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [8]). This underscores the value of repeated testing and provides some perspective in interpreting data collected from a single night of sleep. Additionally, both the presence and overall spread of this within subject variation in certain populations is an outcome that is worth investigating in more depth as an indicator of sleep stability.

    When presented in context with the three aforementioned articles reviewing the function of sleep, we found the work by Cote et al. especially interesting given their analysis of individual differences in sleep patterns and the relationship to waking cognitive function. They found that patients with more severe traumatic brain injury generated fewer evoked k-complexes (in response to auditory stimuli) without influencing spontaneous k-complex production. Injury severity was also negatively correlated to the amount of spindles produced (spindle density) [6]. This is consistent with other reports that sleep features may be associated with injury severity and functional outcomes after TBI [4].

    By factoring injury severity and relevant properties of the observed sleep landscape into a multiple regression analysis, Cote and colleagues were able to conclude that spindle activity in patients with TBI predicted better performance on working memory and general cognitive functioning tests above and beyond the influence of the brain injury alone. This work provides a refreshing perspective to the field by examining how specific sleep events contribute to cognitive functioning.

    It would be interesting to see how the phasic sleep events studied by Cote et al., specifically density of spindles and k-complexes, influence waking cognition in other patient populations. Although the immediate cognitive effects of sleep deprivation were reviewed in detail and the now well-established role of sleep in memory consolidation was succinctly described in Assefa et al. (2015), the potential use of sleep markers to predict cognitive performance at later times (an approach utilized by Cote et al.) was mostly overlooked in the review articles (but see [3]) and is an area of research that, when fully established, could allow us to uncover potential biomarkers of disease. Determining how the EEG morphology of phasic sleep events is related to waking cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders is an exciting area of research that deserves more attention in future studies.


    3. Studying Sleep Structure across Development to Inform Function

    The structure of sleep across the lifespan is always changing and the functional role of sleep events may differ in infancy compared to adulthood. Given the focus on the mechanisms of sleep regulation and health outcomes associated with sleep presented in the current set of reviews, it is surprising that age-related sleep changes were not discussed in more detail. Changes in the macrostructure of sleep have been well described over the past several decades [9,10,11,12,13] and sleep disturbances are a common complaint among elderly patients. Reductions in slow wave sleep and total sleep time, combined with frequent night awakenings characterize the sleep structure of the elderly. As humans age, the inhibitory phasic sleep events discussed in detail by Cote and colleagues (k-complexes and spindles) start to decline [14,15] and intra-individual variation across nights begins to increase [7,15].

    Many neurodevelopmental disorders, including the autism spectrum disorders and ADHD, can be characterized by cognitive deficits in the same tests of working memory (e.g. digit span task) and general cognitive ability (e.g. digit symbol coding task) utilized in the research of Cote and colleagues. However, the role of sleep early in development was only mentioned in passing in two of the review articles [2,3] and not mentioned at all in the third review [1] published in the past 4 issues of AIMS Neuroscience. Normal sleep development includes the appearance of spindles during the first few months of life [16,17] in a pattern that matures along with the development of the thalamo-cortical neural circuitry [18,19] and may be an indicator of normal neural development [17,20,21,22,23]. Human and non-human animal research that investigates how phasic sleep parameters predict neurocognitive development in young subjects may help to considerably advance our understanding of prominent neurodevelopmental disorders and provide further understanding of how sleep shapes the developing brain.


    [1] Assefa SZ, Diaz-Abad M, Wickwire EM, et al. (2015) The functions of sleep. AIMS Neurosci 2: 155-171. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.3.155
    [2] Zielinski MR, McKenna JT, McCarley RW (2016) Functions and Mechanisms of Sleep. AIMS Neurosci 3: 67-104.
    [3] Barone D, Krieger A (2015) The Function of Sleep. AIMS Neurosci 2: 71-90. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.2.71
    [4] Sandsmark DK, Kumar MA, Woodward CS, et al. (2016) Sleep features on continuous electroencephalography predict rehabilitation outcomes after severe traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabilit 31: 101-107. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000217
    [5] Orff HJ, Ayalon L, Drummond SP (2009) Traumatic brain injury and sleep disturbance: a review of current research. J Head Trauma Rehabilit 24: 155-165. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181a0b281
    [6] Cote KA, Milner CE, Speth TA (2015) Altered sleep mechanisms following traumatic brain injury and relation to waking function. AIMS Neurosci 2: 203-228. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.4.203
    [7] Crowley K, Trinder J, Kim Y, et al. (2002) The effects of normal aging on sleep spindle and K-complex production. Clinic Neurophysiol 113: 1615-1622. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00237-7
    [8] Gruber R, Sadeh A, Raviv A (2000) Instability of sleep patterns in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Academy Child Adolescent Psychiatry 39: 495-501. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200004000-00019
    [9] Carskadon MA, Brown ED, Dement WC (1983) Sleep fragmentation in the elderly: relationship to daytime sleep tendency. Neurobiology Aging 3: 321-327.
    [10] Roffwarg HP, Muzio JN, Dement WC (1966) Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science 152: 604-619. doi: 10.1126/science.152.3722.604
    [11] Feinberg I, Koresko RL, Heller N (1967) EEG sleep patterns as a function of normal and pathological aging in man. J Psychiatric Res 5: 107-144.
    [12] Hayashi Y, Endo S (1981) All-night sleep polygraphic recordings of healthy aged persons: REM and slow-wave sleep. Sleep 5: 277-283.
    [13] Rowe JW, Kahn RL (1987) Human aging: usual and successful. Science 237: 143-149. doi: 10.1126/science.3299702
    [14] Nicolas A, Petit D, Rompre S, et al. (2001) Sleep spindle characteristics in healthy subjects of different age groups. Clinic Neurophysiol 112: 521-527. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00556-3
    [15] Wauquier A (1993) Aging and changes in phasic events during sleep. Physiology Behavior 54: 803-806. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90095-W
    [16] Ellingson RJ (1981) Development of sleep spindle bursts during the first year of life. Sleep 5: 39-46.
    [17] Tanguay PE, Ornitz EM, Kaplan A, et al. (1975) Evolution of sleep spindles in childhood. Electroencephalography Clinic Neurophysiol 38: 175-181. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(75)90227-8
    [18] Miyamoto H, Katagiri H, Hensch T (2003) Experience-dependent slow-wave sleep development. Nat Neurosci 6.
    [19] Steriade M, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ (1993) Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science 262: 679-685. doi: 10.1126/science.8235588
    [20] Schultz MA, Schulte FJ, Akiyama Y, et al. (1968) Development of electroencephalographic sleep phenomena in hypothyroid infants. Electroencephalography Clinic Neurophysiol 25: 351-358. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(68)90176-4
    [21] Tiriac A, Blumberg MS (2016) The Case of the Disappearing Spindle Burst. Neural Plasticity 2016.
    [22] Shibagaki M, Kiyono S, Watanabe K, et al. (1982) Concurrent occurrence of rapid eye movement with spindle burst during nocturnal sleep in mentally retarded children. Electroencephalography Clinic Neurophysiol 53: 27-35. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(82)90103-1
    [23] Shinomiya S, Nagata K, Takahashi K, et al. (1999) Development of sleep spindles in young children and adolescents. Clinic EEG Neurosci 30: 39-43. doi: 10.1177/155005949903000203
  • This article has been cited by:

    1. Danielle K. Sandsmark, Jonathan E. Elliott, Miranda M. Lim, Sleep-Wake Disturbances After Traumatic Brain Injury: Synthesis of Human and Animal Studies, 2017, 0161-8105, 10.1093/sleep/zsx044
  • Reader Comments
  • © 2016 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(5333) PDF downloads(1166) Cited by(1)

Article outline

Other Articles By Authors

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog