Research article Topical Sections

Protective behaviors during COVID-19 confinement measures in Greece: the role of anxiety, perceived risk and risky-choice framing

  • Confinement measures at the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic imposed major changes on the global population. The aim of this study was to explore the level to which the public adhered to protective guidelines by adopting the most appropriate behaviors at that time (such as hand washing with soap and using sanitizer gel) and to identify the determinants of these behaviors. A purposive sample of 1013 individuals was invited and voluntarily participated in the online survey. The questionnaire collected information on demographic data, hand washing, risk perception, anxiety (through the S = Anxiety scale of STAI) and risky-choice framing. Results showed increased levels of anxiety, a moderate perception of the risk of catching coronavirus and increased adoption of protective behaviors, such as handwashing and cleaning surfaces with disinfectant/antiseptic products. Multiple ordinal logistic regression models showed that being female, more educated and cleaning home with disinfectant / antiseptic products predicted handwashing with soap. Additionally, having an increased perception of getting the coronavirus, being older and cleaning the home with disinfectant / antiseptic products predicted handwashing with antiseptics. Public health interventions should take into consideration the unified cleaning pattern and the combined effect of sociodemographic variables and risk perception on the adoption of protective behaviour in the context of a health crisis which is out of people's control.

    Citation: George Koulierakis, Anastasia Dermatis, Dimitris Zavras, Elpida Pavi. Protective behaviors during COVID-19 confinement measures in Greece: the role of anxiety, perceived risk and risky-choice framing[J]. AIMS Public Health, 2023, 10(2): 281-296. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2023021

    Related Papers:

    [1] Stephanie A. Godleski, Casey T. Harris, Kevin M. Fitzpatrick, Ammina Kothari . Social and behavioral vulnerability, pregnancy, and negative mental health outcomes in the U.S. during the Covid-19 pandemic. AIMS Public Health, 2022, 9(2): 331-341. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2022023
    [2] Carlee Bellapigna, Zornitsa Kalibatseva . Psychosocial risk factors associated with social anxiety, depressive and disordered eating symptoms during COVID-19. AIMS Public Health, 2023, 10(1): 18-34. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2023003
    [3] Thiresia Sikioti, Afroditi Zartaloudi, Despoina Pappa, Polyxeni Mangoulia, Evangelos C. Fradelos, Freideriki Eleni Kourti, Ioannis Koutelekos, Evangelos Dousis, Nikoletta Margari, Areti Stavropoulou, Eleni Evangelou, Chrysoula Dafogianni . Stress and burnout among Greek critical care nurses during the COVID-19 pandemic. AIMS Public Health, 2023, 10(4): 755-774. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2023051
    [4] Yosef Mohamed-Azzam Zakout, Fayez Saud Alreshidi, Ruba Mustafa Elsaid, Hussain Gadelkarim Ahmed . The magnitude of COVID-19 related stress, anxiety and depression associated with intense mass media coverage in Saudi Arabia. AIMS Public Health, 2020, 7(3): 664-678. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2020052
    [5] José Miguel Uribe-Restrepo, Alan Waich-Cohen, Laura Ospina-Pinillos, Arturo Marroquín Rivera, Sergio Castro-Díaz, Juan Agustín Patiño-Trejos, Martín Alonso Rondón Sepúlveda, Karen Ariza-Salazar, Luisa Fernanda Cardona-Porras, Carlos Gómez-Restrepo, Francisco Diez-Canseco . Mental health and psychosocial impact of the COVID-19 pandemic and social distancing measures among young adults in Bogotá, Colombia. AIMS Public Health, 2022, 9(4): 630-643. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2022044
    [6] Bahar Kefeli Çol, Ayşe Gümüşler Başaran, Hilal Pekmezci . COVID-19 pandemic: Evaluating its psychological impact and individuals' depression, anxiety, and stress levels. AIMS Public Health, 2023, 10(3): 507-523. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2023036
    [7] Abdulqadir J. Nashwan, Rejo G. Mathew, Reni Anil, Nabeel F. Allobaney, Sindhumole Krishnan Nair, Ahmed S. Mohamed, Ahmad A. Abujaber, Abbas Balouchi, Evangelos C. Fradelos . The safety, health, and well-being of healthcare workers during COVID-19: A scoping review. AIMS Public Health, 2023, 10(3): 593-609. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2023042
    [8] Aditi Tomar, Mandy N Spadine, Taylor Graves-Boswell, Lisa T Wigfall . COVID-19 among LGBTQ+ individuals living with HIV/AIDS: psycho-social challenges and care options. AIMS Public Health, 2021, 8(2): 303-308. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2021023
    [9] Robin M. Kowalski, Kenzie Hurley, Nicholas Deas, Sophie Finnell, Kelly Evans, Chelsea Robbins, Andrew Cook, Emily Radovic, Hailey Carroll, Lyndsey Brewer, Gabriela Mochizuki . Protection motivation unmasked: Applying protection motivation theory to skepticism toward COVID-19 mask and vaccine mandates. AIMS Public Health, 2022, 9(3): 506-520. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2022035
    [10] Theodoros Pesiridis, Petros Galanis, Eleni Anagnostopoulou, Athena Kalokerinou, Panayota Sourtzi . Providing care to patients with COVID-19 in a reference hospital: health care staff intentional behavior and factors that affect it. AIMS Public Health, 2021, 8(3): 456-466. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2021035
  • Confinement measures at the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic imposed major changes on the global population. The aim of this study was to explore the level to which the public adhered to protective guidelines by adopting the most appropriate behaviors at that time (such as hand washing with soap and using sanitizer gel) and to identify the determinants of these behaviors. A purposive sample of 1013 individuals was invited and voluntarily participated in the online survey. The questionnaire collected information on demographic data, hand washing, risk perception, anxiety (through the S = Anxiety scale of STAI) and risky-choice framing. Results showed increased levels of anxiety, a moderate perception of the risk of catching coronavirus and increased adoption of protective behaviors, such as handwashing and cleaning surfaces with disinfectant/antiseptic products. Multiple ordinal logistic regression models showed that being female, more educated and cleaning home with disinfectant / antiseptic products predicted handwashing with soap. Additionally, having an increased perception of getting the coronavirus, being older and cleaning the home with disinfectant / antiseptic products predicted handwashing with antiseptics. Public health interventions should take into consideration the unified cleaning pattern and the combined effect of sociodemographic variables and risk perception on the adoption of protective behaviour in the context of a health crisis which is out of people's control.



    The SARS-COV-2 pandemic has been an acute public health threat [1]. It started in January 2020, when the World Health Organization declared the novel acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus (2019-nCoV) to be a Public Health Emergency of international concern [2]. As of September 28, 2022, 613,410,706 confirmed COVID-19 cases, including 6,518,749 deaths, have been reported globally [3]. In Greece, the first confirmed COVID-19 case was reported on February 26, 2020, while the first COVID-19-related death was reported on March 12, 2020 [4]. Within two months – up to April 27, 2020, 136 COVID-19-related deaths, among 2,534 laboratory confirmed cases, were recorded [5]. As of September 25, 2022, a total of 4,920,192 cases and 33,111 deaths have been recorded in Greece [6].

    In order to restrict the number of infections and protect individuals, numerous precautionary measures have been implemented around the world since March 2020. During that period, all countries adopted different policies concerning quarantine measures and day-to-day restrictions, while the effectiveness of those policies was assessed to inform governments in responding to future pandemics [7]. In Greece, on February 27, 2020, the government cancelled all carnival celebrations. From March 10, 2020, gradual lockdown decisions were issued concerning all educational institutes, bars, cafes, restaurants, shopping malls, museums, stadiums and athletic settings, retail shops and religious gatherings, until March 23, 2020 when strict measures on the general population's movement were taken, imposing penalties for breach. All measures aimed at limiting down non-essential movements in an attempt to achieve physical (social) distancing. In parallel, an intensive “Staying at Home” campaign at the national level was implemented. Alongside the measures, recommendations were also made for individual lifestyle changes, by restricting social, work or school contacts and travel. In Greece, the vast majority of the general population complied with the measures and consequently, Greece's death toll was low. Nevertheless, it has been estimated that the impacts of the outbreak were, as everywhere worldwide, multifaceted and affected many parts of the economic, social and political life of the country [8].

    The long periods of quarantine lead to a significant financial and psychological burden, such as increased levels of anger, confusion and stress, as well as extensive behavioral changes worldwide [9][16]. In Portugal, empirical evidence showed that a large amount of the population experienced phobias, fear and anxiety linked to COVID-19, influenced by the perceived risk of contracting COVID-19. The higher the risk of contracting the disease, the higher the phobias, fear and anxiety [17]. A different study in Portugal indicated that individuals expressed fear and anxiety due to COVID-19, related to concerns for tourism, hospitality and travel, as they considered that COVID-19 would influence their leisure time and holidays [18]. At the same time, initiatives to alleviate the burden were adopted [19]. In Greece, previous studies indicated that the public experienced high levels (35.7%) of COVID-19-related fear, moderate to severe (22.8%) depressive symptoms, as well as moderate to severe (77.4%) anxiety symptoms [20]. Increased levels of depression, anxiety, stress and negative affect were found in another study among students, while life disruption, perceived risk of the disease and female gender were risk factors for mental health problems [21]. Additionally, a recent study in Greece demonstrated a strong association of COVID-19 fear with increased levels of health stress, anxiety, and posttraumatic stress among the population [22].

    During the preliminary phase of the pandemic, mass media announced daily COVID-19 mortality and morbidity rates [23]. The exposure to mortality salience and the conscious conception of dying led to an increase in self-protective behaviors such as sanitizing or handwashing, in order to minimize the risk of contracting the deadly virus [24], although findings were not always consistent [25]. Despite the fact that adhering to government health policies was crucial, reports showed that individuals' preventive behaviors were insufficient [26],[27]. In Greece, research indicated that 60% of the general population did not have appropriate hand hygiene behavior [28]. Additionally, another study showed that frequent or constant handwashing was reported by 56.7% and 29.8% respectively, while only 16.7% indicated constant use of face masks. Attitudinal and socioeconomic factors critically affected the public's engagement in these preventive behaviors [29].

    Evidently, adherence to public health policies mainly depends on people's perception of COVID-19 and the risks associated with it. Perceived risk is defined as “an individual's subjective evaluation of their risk of a disease or an unfavorable outcome, often linked with performing a certain risky behavior” [30]. Recent research suggests that individuals who feel that they will be harmed by the disease and are at high risk of getting ill show high adaptation to preventive behaviors [31],[32]. Furthermore, numerous studies have indicated that state anxiety is a psychological characteristic which influences the success of public health policies during pandemics, notably hygiene practices and risk communication [33]. Anxiety is a mental state characterized by an intense sense of tension, worry or apprehension, relative to something adverse that might happen in the future. It is distinguished to “state anxiety” defined as a temporary reaction to adverse events, and “trait anxiety”, which is a more stable personality feature [34]. Cognitive behavioral models support that anxiety appears across a continuum, ranging from low to high levels. Low levels of state anxiety have been associated with the notion that an individual is at low risk of contracting the virus, whereas high levels of state anxiety have been associated with the notion that the individual is at high risk of getting ill [35],[36]. Research during the H1N1 pandemic showed that individuals with low levels of anxiety perceived themselves as maintaining a low risk of contracting the disease, thus washed their hands less frequently than those with high levels of anxiety who viewed themselves as being at high risk [37].

    Over several decades, scientists have been interested in the fact that individuals reply differently to contrasting but objectively analogous interpretations of the same problem. Risky-choice framing has been researched by Tversky & Kahneman [38] in their academic work on the Asian Disease Problem (ADP). The ADP incorporates two scenarios, that is either positively (gain) or negatively (loss) framed. Individuals are presented with two choices when 600 people are facing imminent death: In the gain frame, choice A is that 200 individuals will be saved, and choice B consists of a 1/3 probability that all 600 individuals will be saved, with a 2/3 probability that no individuals will be saved. In the loss frame, choice C is that 400 individuals will die and choice D consists of a 1/3 probability that no one will die, with a 2/3 probability that 600 will die. When this problem was introduced in a way that emphasized individuals being saved (framed positively), empirical evidence showed that 71% of the individuals chose the A option. On the contrary, when the problem was introduced in a way that emphasized on those who would lose their life (framed negatively), 78% of the individuals chose the D option [38]. Extended research showed that not only lay individuals but also individuals in the medical profession are vulnerable to this framing effect [39].

    Under such circumstances, the COVID-19 pandemic provided the opportunity to test, in a real-life setting, theories concerning the interactive outcome of framing and situational factors on decision making and risk appraisal [40],[41]. The notion of decision making has been studied to a great extent, incorporating how risky situations influence our decisions and how various circumstances modify the way the risk is evaluated. Acknowledging how emotions guide the influence of problem framing on individuals, decisions can have actual implications. Analogies to pandemic administration, investigating whether choice under risk is associated with complying with healthcare regulations could provide valuable insight into the possibility of which is the framing that can be used to custom messages regarding particular groups of individuals [42].

    Thus, the aim of this study was to explore the level that the general public in Greece - during the early phase of the pandemic - adhered to protective guidelines by adopting the most appropriate behaviors at that time (hand washing with soap and using sanitizer gel) and identify potential cognitive and psychosocial determinants of these behaviors.

    A cross-sectional on-line survey (Exposome@Home|COVID-19 - Greece) was designed and conducted among a sample of the general adult population (≥18 years, who live for at least 1 year in Greece), during the implementation of the physical distancing measures in Greece (May – June 2020). The study was conducted by the Department of Public Health Policy of the School of Public Health of the University of West Attica, in collaboration with the Cyprus International Institute for Environmental and Public Health, Cyprus University of Technology.

    Given the lack of knowledge on similar studies due to the uniqueness of the pandemic emergency and the measures proposed or taken globally, it was not possible to use prior knowledge for sample size estimation. However, a target sample size of 1000 participants/filled questionnaires was expected to provide high coverage of the general Greek population, based on previous experience with the National Surveys “Health and Well-being” which the Department of Health Economics of the former National School of Public Health carried out at regular intervals. A stratified quota sample of the adult population of Greece, based on age, gender and region of residence, was predetermined with a targeted total of 1000 subjects. Eventually, 1013 individuals answered the on-line questionnaire voluntarily participating in the study.

    By employing a market research company, an effective communication strategy was designed for the dissemination of the link of the questionnaire: it was posted on the “Announcements/News” site of the Department of Public Health Policy at the University of West Attica, and was promoted through social media and the internet (Facebook, Twitter, Google) by using legal marketing engines in order to reach the targeted adult population quota profile.

    Basic sociodemographic data was recorded as follows: gender (1 = male, 2 = female); age; financial affordability was assessed through replies to the question “Considering your total family income and your liabilities, which of the following phrases best suits your financial situation at this period?” (1 = “We make do very well”, 2 = “We make do quite well”, 3 = “We make do well”, 4 = “We have some financial difficulties”, 5 = “We have quite a lot of financial difficulties”, 6 = “We have very serious financial difficulties”); marital status (1 = “Not married”, 2 = “living with partner”, 3 = “Married”, 4 = “Widowed (not re-married)”, 5 = “Divorced” (never re-married”), educational level and employment status. Self-reported health was rated on a five-point ordinal scale (1 = “very bad”, 2 = “bad”, 3 = “fair”, 4 = “good”, 5 = “very good”, 6 = “Not sure/don't remember”) in response to the question “How would you rate your health today?”.

    The level of anxiety was measured with the S-Anxiety scale of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI form-Y-1) [43], as it was validated in Greek [44]. The scale consists of twenty statements that evaluate how the respondent feels “right now, at this moment”, at a 4-poin scale: (1) not at all, (2) somewhat, (3) moderately, (4) very much so. Higher scores represent higher levels of anxiety. A cutoff score of 40 is commonly used to define probable clinical levels of anxiety. Cronbach's alpha was 0.94 for the scale, indicating very good reliability.

    Participants' perception of risk was assessed through replies to the question “How likely do you think you are, you personally, to get infected by the coronavirus?” (1 = “Extremely unlikely”, 2 = “Rather unlikely”, 3 = “Neither unlikely, nor likely”, 4 = “Rather likely”, 5 = “Almost certain”, 98 = “Do not know”, 99 = “Not answer”)

    The positive framing of the Asian Disease Problem, adapted to COVID-19 was used as a measure of the risky-choice framing effect. Participants were presented with the following statement: “To address the spread of coronavirus, between the following two hypothetical scenarios of measures, which would you propose to be adopted to prevent more infections?” (1 = “Measures that will definitively save 2,000 people”, 2 = “Measures by which there is 1/3 chance of saving 6000 people and 2/3 chance of saving no one”, 98 = “Do not know”, 99 = “Not answer”).

    Information regarding protective behaviors were obtained by asking participants to report the frequency of adopting one of the following measures: i) washing hands with soap, ii) washing hands with antiseptic products, coded as “Never”, “1–3 times a day”, “4–7 times a day”, “>7 times a day”. Additionally, participants were asked to report the average number of days per week that they were using disinfectant/antiseptic products to clean the sanitary facilities (e.g. bathrooms, toilets), kitchen surfaces, or other places and to mop the floors. Participants were also asked to mention previous preventive behaviors, namely vaccination for influenza (ever) and pneumococcus (during the last 5 years).

    Participation in the study was completely voluntary. Participants were informed about their right not to participate and/or withdraw from the study at any point, without their responses to be kept. Informed consent to participate was provided by clicking the relevant button at the end of the 1st Screen of the on-line questionnaire, where all relevant study information was provided. Respondents were also able to address questions for more information (special e-mail address and telephone number were also provided on the 1st Screen), as well as submit complaints to a staff member of the Department who was involved to the survey (name and e-mail address were given on 1st Screen). The protocol of the study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of West Attica (Decision No. 30548/05–05–2020).

    A descriptive analysis to summarize participants' demographics was applied, using percentages for categorical variables and means and standard deviations for continuous variables and counts. Similarly, we analyzed respondents' COVID-19 related protective behaviors during the confinement measures, their level of anxiety and their perceptions of risk catching the virus. Ordinal logistic regression models were fitted to evaluate the influence of demographic and socioeconomic characteristics, anxiety, perceived risk, risky-choice framing and past behaviors on the reported protective behaviors (hand washing with soap and antiseptics) during the confinement measures. The proportional odds assumption was tested through the Brant test [45]. The models' goodness of fit was tested through the Lipsitz et al. test [46]. In addition, the models were tested for specification error through the link test. The analysis was performed with the STATA 17 statistical software package. Specifically, the commands ologit, brant [47], ologit, gof [48] and linktest were used. All statistical tests were two-sided at 5% significance level.

    Participants' mean age was 48.6 years (SD = 14.9 years). Table 1 shows the basic sociodemographic characteristics of the sample. As presented in Table 1, participants were almost equally divided to males and females, the majority (46.2%) were married, and almost one out of five (24.2%) had completed secondary education. Of the sample, 359 individuals (35.4%) were employed in the private sector, either on a full-time or a part-time basis. More than half of participants (53.2%) reported that they faced financial difficulties to come up with their liabilities.

    Table 1.  Participants' basic demographic characteristics.
    n %
    Gender
    Male 514 50.7
    Female 499 49.3
    Marital status
    Not married 315 31.1
    Living with partner 80 7.9
    Married 468 46.2
    Widowed (not re-married) 47 4.6
    Divorced (never re-married 103 10.2
    Educational level
    Never been at school /Not completed primary education 4 0.4
    Primary education 26 2.6
    Completed secondary education 245 24.2
    Post-secondary education 83 8.2
    Higher technical education (tertiary) 127 12.5
    University 263 26
    Μaster's degree 204 20.1
    Ph.D. degree 61 6
    Employment status
    Employed (public sector) 189 18.7
    Employed (private sector) 359 35.4
    Unemployed 152 15
    Household (no payment) 33 3.3
    Retired 166 16.4
    Student 71 7
    Army service 5 0.5
    Other 38 3.8
    Financial affordability
    We make do very well 58 5.7
    We make do quite well 122 12.1
    We make do well 275 27.3
    We have some financial difficulties 286 28.3
    We have quite a lot financial difficulties 160 15.9
    We have very serious financial difficulties 91 9

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Table 2 presents participants' health status, their perception of the risk of getting COVID-19, and their protective behaviors during the confinement measures. As shown in Table 2, the vast majority of the participants (84.7%) reported a good/very good status of their health. Anxiety levels were high, as the average score of S-Anxiety scale was above the cutoff point of 40, thus indicating potential clinical levels of anxiety. Most of participants (45.6%) were seemed to be risk-averse, as they chose the secure option of the ADP. Almost half of them (49.6%) appeared to be balanced as far as their perception regarding the risk of getting COVID-19, while almost equal percentages reported limited (22.6%) and increased likelihood (23.4%). The majority of respondents (55.1%) had never been vaccinated for influenza, as had an even higher percentage (74.1%) for pneumococcus during the last 5 years. As far as protective behaviors, the majority claimed frequent (“4-7 times a day”: 39.9%) or excessive (“>7 times a day”: 28.5%) handwashing with soap, while only 3.3% mentioned that they never washed their hands. As far as washing hands with antiseptic products, the majority (38.1%) mentioned constant (“1-3 times a day”) practice, while a notable percentage (24.7%) had never used antiseptics to wash their hands. Finally, participants reported an increased average of 4.8 (SD = 2.5) days per week that cleaned their kitchen surfaces with antiseptic products.

    Table 2.  Health status, perceived risk, framing and protective behaviors of the study participants.
    N (%) Mean SD
    Self-reported health status
    Very bad 5 (0.5)
    Bad 20 (2)
    Fair 128 (12.6)
    Good 407 (40.2)
    Very good 451 (44.5)
    Not sure/don't remember 2 (0.2)
    State Anxiety 44.7 13.7
    Risky-choice framing
    Measures that will definitively save 2000 people 462 (45.6)
    Measures by which there is 1/3 chance of saving 6000 people and 2/3 chance of saving no one 115 (11.4)
    Do not know 325 (32.1)
    Not answer 111 (11)
    Perceived risk of getting infected
    Extremely unlikely 89 (8.8)
    Rather unlikely 140 (13.8)
    Neither unlikely, nor likely 502 (49.6)
    Rather likely 212 (20.9)
    Almost certain 25 (2.5)
    Do not know 43 (4.2)
    Not answer 2 (0.2)
    Ever vaccinated for influenza
    YES 390 (38.5)
    NO 558 (55.1)
    Do not know 60 (5.9)
    Not answer 5 (0.5)
    Pneumococcus vaccination during the last 5 years
    YES 190 (18.8)
    NO 751 (74.1)
    Do not know 71 (7)
    Not answer 1 (0.1)
    Washing hands with soap
    Never 33 (3.3)
    1−3 times a day 287 (28.3)
    4−7 times a day 404 (39.9)
    >7 times a day 289 (28.5)
    Washing hands with antiseptic products
    Never 250 (24.7)
    1−3 times a day 386 (38.1)
    4−7 times a day 232 (22.9)
    >7 times a day 145 (14.3)
    Use disinfectant/antiseptic products to... (days/week)
    Clean bathrooms, toilets 3.60 2.5
    Clean kitchen surfaces 4.28 2.5
    Clean other places 2.69 2.2
    Mop the floors 2.65 2.1

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Table 3.  Ordinal Logistic regression of hand-washing with soap and antiseptic products.
    Model 1 - Hand-washing (soap)
    Variables OR (95% CI) SE P
    Gender 0.46 0.36−0.58 0.05 0.000
    Use disinfectant/antiseptic products to clean 1.28 1.21−1.36 0.04 0.000
    Educational level 1.14 1.07−1.22 0.03 0.000
    Model 2 - Hand-washing (antiseptics)
    Risk perception 1.37 1.20−1.56 0.09 0.000
    Use disinfectant/antiseptic products to clean 1.45 1.36−1.55 0.04 0.000
    Age 1.01 1.00−1.02 0.004 0.000

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    According to the link tests (Table 4), both models do not suffer from specification errors.

    Table 4.  Link test. Hand-washing with soap and antiseptic products.
    Model 1 - Hand-washing (soap)
    Variables coefficient P (95% CI)
    h 0.96 0.001 0.373–1.562
    h2 0.01 0.912 −0.191–0.214
    Model 2 - Hand-washing (antiseptics)
    h 0.980 0.018 0.167–1.793
    h2 0.003 0.962 −0.142–0.149

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    The Brant tests indicated that the assumption of proportional odds is not violated: [model 1: p(all) = 0.143, p(use disinfectant products) = 0.079, p(gender) = 0.067, p(educational level) = 0.663], [model 2: p(all) = 0.229, p(perceived risk) = 0.244, p(use disinfectant products) = 0.449, p(age) = 0.097]. The Lipsitz tests indicated that both models had a good fit (model 1: p = 0.34, model 2: p = 0.30).

    The current study investigated the extent to which the general public in Greece adhered to protective guidelines by practicing the most appropriate health behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic, namely hand washing with soap and sanitizer gel. Additionally, this study aimed to identify potential cognitive and psychosocial determinants of these behaviors. It has been widely documented that the SAR-COV-2 pandemic had a significant impact on public health [1]. Countries around the world adopted preventive measures in order to limit the number of infections, and to protect individuals from the deadly virus. Since March 2020, strict government measures have been enforced globally, regarding quarantine protocols and daily restrictions [7], intended to attain social distancing in order to reduce contamination [9]. In Greece, confinement measures were gradually posed since February 2020 when the Greek government canceled the traditional carnival festivities, followed by a strict lock-down regulations imposed on the general population on March 23, 2020.

    During the long periods of social isolation, the general public experienced acute levels of psychological impact. Individuals reported increased levels of anxiety, depression, and fear related to COVID-19. Results of the current study indicated that during the primary quarantine phase of the COVID-19 pandemic, anxiety levels were high in Greece (the average S-Anxiety scale score was 44.7, that is over the cutoff point of 40), approaching clinical levels. Thus, our findings confirm previous studies in Greece, which showed that during the long periods of social isolation, the general public experienced increased levels of anxiety, depression and fear related to COVID-19 [20][22]. Our results are also compatible with studies showing that the prolonged phase of quarantine led to significant psychological effects in different countries worldwide [9],[10],[15],[17],[18].

    As for how risk perception framing affects people's decision making, our results showed that 45.6% of the participants were opposed to risk and chose the secure option when faced with the Asian Disease Problem, thus confirming findings by Rachev et al. who suggested that high risk aversion is associated with elevated levels of anxiety [42]. Further scientific evidence indicates that increased levels of anxiety are linked to people's disposition to refrain from risky decision making [49]. Moreover, our results are in line findings reporting that anxiety and distress have a significant effect on decision making than other determinants, such as income [50].

    As indicated by scores on protective behaviors (handwashing with soap, handwashing with antiseptics), the majority of the participants in this study washed their hands with soap frequently (39.9%), or excessively (28.5%), whereas a high percentage of individuals (38.1%), washed their hands with antiseptic products. Additionally, gender, education level and cleaning the house with disinfectant/antiseptic products were the only determinants of handwashing with soap, while risk perception, cleaning the house using disinfectant/antiseptic products and age predicted handwashing with antiseptics. Previous research has suggested that emotional states (i.e. anxiety, fear) and perceived risk affected compliance with SAR-COV-2 safety recommendations, particularly hand washing [26],[51]. Although, in our study, we did not find anxiety as an independent predictor of hand washing, our findings seemed to confirm the interrelations between risk perception, anxiety and worry, as the most critical determinants of preventive behavior [24]. It seems that people's sense of control could be used as a potent explanation to interpret our data: as people faced at that time a completely unknown and out of their control situation, they adopted the only available measure for their protection [51].

    The role of sociodemographic characteristics such as age, gender and educational level in adopting preventive behaviors and/or adherence to government preventive policies appears constantly in the literature, either as individual associates [29],[52],[53], or in relation to perceived risk [54]. In our study, older individuals, women and those with higher education adopted COVID-19 related protective behaviors more frequently. Evidence has indicated that individuals aged 65 and above are at risk of acute health issues from COVID-19 and death rates among the senior population have been heightened by complications due to the virus [55]. Our findings regarding age are consistent with those of Brankston et al., who documented that perceived risk increased adherence towards preventive behaviors for older individuals [56]. As for gender, women in our study had better adherence towards protective health policies than men. This could be due to gender stereotypes [57] and the fact that women take fewer risks than men [58]. Finally, a strong determinant for adopting frequent COVID-19 protective behaviors in our study was education. Individuals with higher education washed their hands with soap more frequently than those with lower education, thus confirming previous research and is consistent with evidence by Wise et al., indicating that participants with college-level education had greater adherence towards preventive behaviors [59].

    At the same time, participants in our study, seemed to adopt a unified protective behavioral pattern: those who used disinfectant /antiseptic products to clean their houses adopted a more frequent hand washing [60].

    The current study investigated a purposive sample of the Greek general public and was based on a potent methodological design. Although this type of methodology was common at that period onwards and forced by the strict limitations, our research has some limitations. Due to the fact that our on-line survey was structured on self-reported ratings, this could trigger social desirability bias. Furthermore, the cross-sectional nature of our survey prevents the development of casual inferences. Moreover, in order to recruit our participants, a snowball sampling method was used, which means we are not guaranteed a national sample. Thus, generalization cannot be securely assumed.

    Our findings, based on 1013 participants, support significant implications for both practice and theory. High levels of anxiety made participants avoid risk averse choices, as shown in the Asian Disease Problem. Furthermore, perceived risk and other demographic characteristics influenced the adherence to preventive behaviors during the initial COVID-19 quarantine phase in Greece. Hand washing with soap and hand washing with antiseptic products was high during this phase. These behaviors did not seem to be connected with previous protective public health practices like influenza and pneumococcus vaccination, which were reported as low, indicating the unique characteristics of the COVID-19 pandemic. Further research should be conducted on the fundamental processes through which COVID-19 mortality and morbidity approaches may influence protective behaviors. Such processes may involve people's unconscious operations, behavioral intentions and understanding of COVID-19 transmission. Knowing the cognitive and psychosocial determinants of protective behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic could help protect the general public, develop efficient preventive actions and target communication strategies in order to vigorously participate in the fight against the deadly virus.


    Acknowledgments



    We would like to thank all participants in this survey. The authors also thank the Laboratory for Health Technology Assessment – LabHTA, which funded, by own funds, the electronic formulation of the questionnaire and data collection through the Special Account for Research Grants - University of West Attica, code no. 80712.

    Conflict of interest



    The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

    [1] Grover S, Sahoo S, Mehra A, et al. (2020) Psychological impact of COVID-19 lockdown: An online survey from India. Indian J Psychiatry 62: 354-362. https://doi.org/10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_427_20
    [2] World Health Organization (WHO) WHO Director-General's statement on IHR Emergency Committee on Novel Coronavirus (2019-nCoV).
    [3] World Health Organization (WHO) WHO Coronavirus (COVID-19) Dashboard. Available from: https://covid19.who.int/.
    [4] National Public Health Organization Current state of Covid-19 outbreak in Greece and timeline of key containment events. Available from: https://eody.gov.gr/en/current-state-of-covid-19-outbreak-in-greece-and-timeline-of-key-containment-events/.
    [5] National Public Health Organization Epidemiological Report (COVID-19). Available from: https://eody.gov.gr/wp-content/uploads/2020/04/covid-gr-daily-report-20200427.pdf.
    [6] National Public Health Organization Weekly Epidemiological Report on SARS-COV-2 (19–25 September 2022). Available from: https://eody.gov.gr/wp-content/uploads/2022/09/covid-gr-weekly-report-2022-38.pdf.
    [7] Dergiades T, Milas C, Mossialos E, et al. (2022) Effectiveness of government policies in response to the first COVID-19 outbreak. PLOS Glob Public Health 2: e0000242. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgph.0000242
    [8] Kousi T, Mitsi LC, Simos J (2021) The Early Stage of COVID-19 Outbreak in Greece: A Review of the National Response and the Socioeconomic Impact. Int J Env Res Public Health 18: 322. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18010322
    [9] Rodríguez-Fernández P, González-Santos J, Santamaría-Peláez M, et al. (2021) Psychological Effects of Home Confinement and Social Distancing Derived from COVID-19 in the General Population-A Systematic Review. Int J Environ Res Public Health 18: 6528. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126528
    [10] Chinna K, Sundarasen S, Khoshaim HB, et al. (2021) Psychological impact of COVID-19 and lock down measures: An online cross-sectional multicounty study on Asian university students. PLoS One 16: e0253059. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253059
    [11] Sang X, Menhas R, Saqib ZA, et al. (2021) The Psychological Impacts of COVID-19 Home Confinement and Physical Activity: A Structural Equation Model Analysis. Front Psychol 11: 614770. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.614770
    [12] Uribe-Restrepo JM, Waich-Cohen A, Ospina-Pinillos L, et al. (2022) Mental health and psychosocial impact of the COVID-19 pandemic and social distancing measures among young adults in Bogotá, Colombia. AIMS Public Health 9: 630-643. https://doi.org/10.3934/publichealth.2022044
    [13] Magdy DM, Metwally A, Magdy O (2022) Assessment of community psycho-behavioral responses during the outbreak of novel coronavirus (2019-nCoV): a cross-sectional study. AIMS Public Health 9: 26-40. https://doi.org/10.3934/publichealth.2022003
    [14] Schelhorn I, Ecker A, Lüdtke MN, et al. (2021) Psychological Burden During the COVID-19 Pandemic in Germany. Front Psycholy 12: 640518. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.640518
    [15] Favieri F, Forte G, Tambelli R, et al. (2021) The Italians in the Time of Coronavirus: Psychosocial Aspects of the Unexpected COVID-19 Pandemic. Front Psychol 12: 551924. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.551924
    [16] Zavras D (2021) A cross-sectional population-based study on the influence of the COVID-19 pandemic on incomes in Greece. AIMS Public Health 8: 376-387. https://doi.org/10.3934/publichealth.2021029
    [17] Leite Â, Vidal DG, Sousa HFPe, et al. (2021) Portuguese version of COVID-19 perceived risk scale and COVID-19 phobia scale: Psychometric properties. Eur J Invest Health 11: 1044-1060. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe11030078
    [18] Magano J, Vidal DG, Sousa HFPe, et al. (2021) Validation and psychometric properties of the Portuguese version of the Coronavirus Anxiety Scale (CAS) and Fear of COVID-19 Scale (FCV-19S) and associations with travel, tourism and hospitality. Int J Environ Res Public Health 18: 427. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18020427
    [19] Fazia T, Bubbico F, Nova A, et al. (2022) Online Short-Term Mindfulness-Based Intervention During COVID-19 Quarantine in Italy: Effects on Wellbeing, Stress, and Anxiety. Front Psychol 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.914183
    [20] Parlapani E, Holeva V, Voitsidis P, et al. (2020) Psychological and Behavioral Responses to the COVID-19 Pandemic in Greece. Front Psychol 11: 821. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00821
    [21] Kornilaki Α (2022) The psychological consequences of COVID-19 pandemic on University students in Greece. The role of daily activities during the quarantine. Psychology 26: 144-164.
    [22] Nikopoulou VA, Holeva V, Parlapani E, et al. (2022) Mental health screening for COVID-19: A proposed cutoff score for the Greek version of the Fear of COVID-19 Scale (FCV-19S). Int J of Ment Health Addict 20: 907-920. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00414-w
    [23] Gozzi N, Tizzani M, Starnini M, et al. (2020) Collective Response to Media Coverage of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Reddit and Wikipedia: Mixed-Methods Analysis. J Med Internet Res 22: e21597. https://doi.org/10.2196/21597
    [24] Sobkow A, Zaleskiewicz T, Petrova D, et al. (2020) Worry, Risk Perception, and Controllability Predict Intentions Toward COVID-19 Preventive Behaviors. Front Psychol 11: 582720-582720. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.582720
    [25] Szczuka Z, Abraham C, Baban A, et al. (2021) The trajectory of COVID-19 pandemic and handwashing adherence: findings from 14 countries. BMC Public Health 21: 1791-1791. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11822-5
    [26] Chen X, Ran L, Liu Q, et al. (2020) Hand Hygiene, Mask-Wearing Behaviors and Its Associated Factors during the COVID-19 Epidemic: A Cross-Sectional Study among Primary School Students in Wuhan, China. Int J Environ Res Public Health 17: 2893. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17082893
    [27] Nivette A, Ribeaud D, Murray A, et al. (2021) Non-compliance with COVID-19-related public health measures among young adults in Switzerland: Insights from a longitudinal cohort study. Soc Sci Med 268: 11337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113370
    [28] Mouchtouri VA, Agathagelidou E, Kofonikolas K, et al. (2020) Nationwide Survey in Greece about Knowledge, Risk Perceptions, and Preventive Behaviors for COVID-19 during the General Lockdown in April 2020. Int J Environ Res Public Health 17: 8854. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17238854
    [29] Souliotis K, Giannouchos TV, Peppou LE, et al. (2021) Public Health Behaviors during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Greece and Associated Factors: A Nationwide Cross-sectional Survey. Inquiry 58: 469580211022913. https://doi.org/10.1177/00469580211022913
    [30] Encyclopedia of behavioral medicine. Cham: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-39903-0
    [31] Lammers J, Crusius J, Gast A (2020) Correcting misperceptions of exponential coronavirus growth increases support for social distancing. P Natl Acad Sci 117: 16264-16266. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2006048117
    [32] Shiina A, Niitsu T, Kobori O, et al. (2020) Relationship between perception and anxiety about COVID-19 infection and risk behaviors for spreading infection: A national survey in Japan. Brain Behav Immun Health 6: 10010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbih.2020.100101
    [33] Taylor S (2022) The Psychology of Pandemics. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 18: 581-609. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-072720-020131
    [34] Saviola F, Pappaianni E, Monti A, et al. (2020) Trait and state anxiety are mapped differently in the human brain. Sci Rep 10: 11112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-68008-z
    [35] Asmundson GJG, Abramowitz JS, Richter AA, et al. (2010) Health Anxiety: Current Perspectives and Future Directions. Curr Psychiat Rep 12: 306-312. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-010-0123-9
    [36] Asmundson GJG, Taylor S, Nicholas Carleton R, et al. (2012) Should health anxiety be carved at the joint? A look at the health anxiety construct using factor mixture modeling in a non-clinical sample. J Anxiety Disord 26: 246-251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.11.009
    [37] Gilles I, Bangerter A, Clémence A, et al. (2011) Trust in medical organizations predicts pandemic (H1N1) 2009 vaccination behavior and perceived efficacy of protection measures in the Swiss public. Eur J Epidemiol 26: 203-210. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-011-9577-2
    [38] Tversky A, Kahneman D (1981) The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice. Science 211: 453-458. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683
    [39] McNeil BJ, Pauker SG, Sox HC, et al. (1982) On the Elicitation of Preferences for Alternative Therapies. N Engl J Med 306: 1259-1262. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJM198205273062103
    [40] Mandel DR (2014) Do framing effects reveal irrational choice?. J Exp Psychol Gen 143: 1185-1198. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034207
    [41] Chick CF, Reyna VF, Corbin JC (2016) Framing effects are robust to linguistic disambiguation: A critical test of contemporary theory. J Exp Psychol Learn 42: 238-256. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000158
    [42] Rachev NR, Han H, Lacko D, et al. (2021) Replicating the Disease framing problem during the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic: A study of stress, worry, trust, and choice under risk. PLoS One 16: e0257151. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257151
    [43] Spielberger CD (1970) Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (Self-evaluation questionnare).Consulting Psychogyists Press.
    [44] Fountoulakis KN, Papadopoulou M, Kleanthous S, et al. (2006) Reliability and psychometric properties of the Greek translation of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory form Y: preliminary data. Ann Gen Psychiatry 5: 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-859X-5-2
    [45] Brant R (1990) Assessing Proportionality in the Proportional Odds Model for Ordinal Logistic Regression. Biometrics 46: 1171-1178. https://doi.org/10.2307/2532457
    [46] Lipsitz SR, Fitzmaurice GM, Molenberghs G (1996) Goodness-of-Fit Tests for Ordinal Response Regression Models. J R Stat Soc C-Appl 45: 175-190. https://doi.org/10.2307/2986153
    [47] Long JS, Freese J Regression models for categorical dependent variables using stata, Stata Press (2014).
    [48] Fagerland MW, Hosmer DW (2017) How to Test for Goodness of Fit in Ordinal Logistic Regression Models. Stata J 17: 668-686. https://doi.org/10.1177/1536867X1701700308
    [49] Xu F, Huang L (2022) The Influence of Trait Emotion and Spatial Distance on Risky Choice Under the Framework of Gain and Loss. Front Psychol 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.592584
    [50] Cicerale A, Blanzieri E, Sacco K (2022) How does decision-making change during challenging times?. PLoS One 17: e0270117. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0270117
    [51] Li P, Zhu H (2022) Sense of Control and Safety Compliance in the Prevention of COVID-19: A Framework Based on Conservation of Resources Theory. Front Psychol 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.790459
    [52] Dev R, Raparelli V, Bacon SL, et al. (2022) Impact of biological sex and gender-related factors on public engagement in protective health behaviours during the COVID-19 pandemic: cross-sectional analyses from a global survey. BMJ Open 12: e059673. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2021-059673
    [53] Tan J, Yoshida Y, Ma KS-K, et al. (2021) Gender Differences in Health Protective Behaviors During the COVID-19 Pandemic in Taiwan: An Empirical Study. medRxiv . https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.04.14.21255448
    [54] Bruine de Bruin W (2021) Age Differences in COVID-19 Risk Perceptions and Mental Health: Evidence From a National U.S. Survey Conducted in March 2020. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 76: e24-e29. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa074
    [55] Ssentongo P, Ssentongo AE, Heilbrunn ES, et al. (2020) Association of cardiovascular disease and 10 other pre-existing comorbidities with COVID-19 mortality: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 15: e0238215. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238215
    [56] Brankston G, Merkley E, Fisman DN, et al. (2021) Socio-demographic disparities in knowledge, practices, and ability to comply with COVID-19 public health measures in Canada. Can J Public Health 112: 363-375. https://doi.org/10.17269/s41997-021-00501-y
    [57] Capraro V, Barcelo H (2020) The effect of messaging and gender on intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission. J Behav Econ Policy 4: 45-55. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/tg7vz
    [58] Pawlowski B, Atwal R, Dunbar RIM (2008) Sex Differences in Everyday Risk-Taking Behavior in Humans. Evol Psychol 6: 147470490800600100. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470490800600104
    [59] Wise T, Zbozinek TD, Michelini G, et al. (2020) Changes in risk perception and self-reported protective behaviour during the first week of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. R Soc Open Sci 7: 200742-200742. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.200742
    [60] Brown LG, Hoover ER, Barrett CE, et al. (2020) Handwashing and disinfection precautions taken by U.S. adults to prevent coronavirus disease 2019, Spring 2020. BMC Res Notes 13: 550. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-020-05398-3
  • Reader Comments
  • © 2023 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(1837) PDF downloads(90) Cited by(0)

Figures and Tables

Tables(4)

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog