Research article

Brain dynamics and personality: a preliminary study

  • Francesco Ciaramella and Lorenzo Cipriano contributed equally to this work as “first authors”.
  • Received: 10 September 2024 Revised: 02 December 2024 Accepted: 09 December 2024 Published: 12 December 2024
  • Personality can be considered a system characterized by complex dynamics that are extremely adaptive depending on continuous interactions with the environment and situations. The present preliminary study explores the dynamic interplay between brain flexibility and personality by taking the dynamic approach to personality into account, thereby drawing from Cloninger's psychobiological model. 46 healthy individuals were recruited, and their brain dynamics were assessed using magnetoencephalography (MEG) during the resting state. We identified brain activation patterns and measured brain flexibility by employing the theory of neuronal avalanches. Subsequent correlation analyses revealed a significant positive association between brain flexibility and cooperativeness, thus highlighting the role of brain reconfiguration tendencies in fostering openness, tolerance, and empathy towards others. Additionally, this preliminary finding suggests a neurobiological basis for adaptive social behaviors. Although the results are preliminary, this study provides initial insights into the intricate relationship between brain dynamics and personality, thus laying the groundwork for further research in this emerging field using a dynamic network analysis of the functional activity of the brain.

    Citation: Francesco Ciaramella, Lorenzo Cipriano, Emahnuel Troisi Lopez, Arianna Polverino, Fabio Lucidi, Giuseppe Sorrentino, Laura Mandolesi, Pierpaolo Sorrentino. Brain dynamics and personality: a preliminary study[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(4): 490-504. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024030

    Related Papers:

    [1] Anna Lardone, Marianna Liparoti, Pierpaolo Sorrentino, Roberta Minino, Arianna Polverino, Emahnuel Troisi Lopez, Simona Bonavita, Fabio Lucidi, Giuseppe Sorrentino, Laura Mandolesi . Topological changes of brain network during mindfulness meditation: an exploratory source level magnetoencephalographic study. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(2): 250-263. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022013
    [2] Ebtesam M Abd-El-Basset, Muddanna Sakkattu Rao, Solaiman M Alshawaf, Hasan Kh Ashkanani, Abdulaziz H Kabli . Tumor necrosis factor (TNF) induces astrogliosis, microgliosis and promotes survival of cortical neurons. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(4): 558-584. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021031
    [3] Shun Sakuma, Yuko Mizuno-Matsumoto, Yoshi Nishitani, Shinichi Tamura . Simulation of Spike Wave Propagation and Two-to-one Communication with Dynamic Time Warping. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(4): 474-486. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.4.474
    [4] Pranav Chintamani Joshi, Sugato Benerjee . Effects of glucocorticoids in depression: Role of astrocytes. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(3): 200-210. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.3.200
    [5] Yoshi Nishitani, Chie Hosokawa, Yuko Mizuno-Matsumoto, Tomomitsu Miyoshi, Shinichi Tamura . Classification of Spike Wave Propagations in a Cultured Neuronal Network: Investigating a Brain Communication Mechanism. AIMS Neuroscience, 2017, 4(1): 1-13. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.1.1
    [6] Annette Karmiloff-Smith . An Alternative to Domain-general or Domain-specific Frameworks for Theorizing about Human Evolution and Ontogenesis. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(2): 91-104. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.2.91
    [7] Ian M Rowbotham, Franco F Orsucci, Mohamed F Mansour, Samuel R Chamberlain, Haroon Y Raja . Relevance of Brain-derived Neurotrophic Factor Levels in Schizophrenia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(4): 280-293. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.4.280
    [8] I. Banegas, I. Prieto, A.B. Segarra, M. de Gasparo, M. Ramírez-Sánchez . Study of the Neuropeptide Function in Parkinson’s Disease Using the 6-Hydroxydopamine Model of Experimental Hemiparkinsonism. AIMS Neuroscience, 2017, 4(4): 223-237. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.4.223
    [9] mgr Natalia Białoń, dr hab. n. o zdr. Dariusz Górka, mgr Mikołaj Górka . The brain-gut axis: communication mechanisms and the role of the microbiome as a neuroprotective factor in the development of neurodegenerative diseases: A literature overview. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(3): 289-311. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024019
    [10] Gigi Tevzadze, Tamar Barbakadze, Elisabed Kvergelidze, Elene Zhuravliova, Lali Shanshiashvili, David Mikeladze . Gut neurotoxin p-cresol induces brain-derived neurotrophic factor secretion and increases the expression of neurofilament subunits in PC-12 cells. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(1): 12-23. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022002
  • Personality can be considered a system characterized by complex dynamics that are extremely adaptive depending on continuous interactions with the environment and situations. The present preliminary study explores the dynamic interplay between brain flexibility and personality by taking the dynamic approach to personality into account, thereby drawing from Cloninger's psychobiological model. 46 healthy individuals were recruited, and their brain dynamics were assessed using magnetoencephalography (MEG) during the resting state. We identified brain activation patterns and measured brain flexibility by employing the theory of neuronal avalanches. Subsequent correlation analyses revealed a significant positive association between brain flexibility and cooperativeness, thus highlighting the role of brain reconfiguration tendencies in fostering openness, tolerance, and empathy towards others. Additionally, this preliminary finding suggests a neurobiological basis for adaptive social behaviors. Although the results are preliminary, this study provides initial insights into the intricate relationship between brain dynamics and personality, thus laying the groundwork for further research in this emerging field using a dynamic network analysis of the functional activity of the brain.



    The complex interaction between genetic and environmental factors induces changes in the brain that inevitably affect individuality and behavioral choices [1]. In the last decade, personality neuroscience has emerged as a new field of investigating personality and individual differences [2][4]. The goal is to understand the relationship between the brain and personality, mainly in the general population [5]. In this framework, personality is considered in relation to neurobiological correlates, and understanding brain functioning is the basis of knowledge of the characteristics regulating behavior and becomes a valid prerequisite for predicting it.

    In a previous magnetoencephalography study, by taking the perspective of the brain as an interconnected network into account [6], we used Cloninger's psychobiological model to study how individual differences are correlated with specific cerebral structures [7]. Using the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) [8], this model identified four primary-basic personality temperaments (Novelty Seeking, NS; Harm Avoidance, HA; Reward Dependence, RD; and Persistence, P) and three characters (Self-Directedness, SD; Cooperativeness, CO; and Self-Transcendence, ST). In particular, during a resting state condition, we observed high HA scores that were associated with a reduced centrality of the left caudate nucleus, which suggested that this nucleus could play a key role in adaptive behavior, probably for its involvement in the insular salience network [7].

    Other studies have focused on understanding the relationships between personality and neurobiological correlates using different theoretical constructs such as the Big Five model/five-factor model [9],[10]. The correlation between personality dimensions and specific cerebral networks has been highlighted, thereby evidencing changes in the volumes of frontal, temporal, and parietal areas as well as of the insula, amygdala, and basal ganglia [11][16]. For example, Aghajani and colleagues [17] evidenced specific functional connectivity patterns originating from the amygdala that correlated to neuroticism and extraversion scores. Additionally, the role of the cerebellum in sustaining motivational temperamental traits has been documented [18][20], thus confirming the insight of the cerebellum on social and affective behaviors [21],[22].

    To date, most studies dealing with brain connectivity and personality have examined the interplay among regions using time-averaged data (static networks), while only a few have explored the time-evolving interaction (among multiple brain areas) that rapidly changes in response to external and internal demands (dynamic networks). The human brain works in a critical regime [23],[24]; to adapt to the surrounding and changing environment, the brain performs continuous and efficient reconfigurations of patterns of activity that reflect the coordinated recruitment of different areas in any circumstance [25],[26]. The more the brain reconfigures itself (i.e., the more flexible), the more appropriate its reply will be to the environmental stimulus [27][29]. This adaptability is mirrored in large-scale bursts of activations in the brain signals, called “neuronal avalanches” [30], which repeatedly remodel the brain functional connectome, giving rise to a large number of patterns (spatio-temporal sequences of regions that are recruited in avalanches) over time. The number of such patterns (i.e., the number of avalanche configurations) is named “functional repertoire” and provides a measure of brain flexibility. The brain propensity to reconfigure itself has been related to several aspects involved in personality architecture, such as executive function, creativity, and resilience [31][34], thus suggesting a role of brain network dynamics in psychological outcomes.

    Recently, personality has been considered a system characterized by complex dynamics that are extremely adaptive depending on continuous interactions with the environment and situations [35]. Therefore, in this context, a dynamic network analysis of the functional activity of the brain proves to be a useful approach to studying personality.

    In this work, we hypothesize that the features of brain dynamics may underlie specific personality characteristics. To verify this hypothesis, we first acquired 46 healthy people by a magnetoencephalography (MEG) system in order to estimate the brain dynamic patterns during resting state. Although it is known that personality studies typically involve larger sample sizes, the use of MEG in this study partly justifies the choice of a smaller sample. However, this constitutes a limitation of this study. Specifically, starting from the reconstructed time series of the brain signals, we identified a neuronal avalanche, which is defined as an event that begins when at least one brain region deviates from its baseline activity and ends when all regions restore their typical level of activity. To identify the avalanches, each source-reconstructed signal was z-scored over time and then dichotomized to 1 (one) if it was above the definite threshold and to 0 (zero) otherwise. Given a neuronal avalanche, we defined its corresponding pattern as the set of all the brain areas that were recruited. Subsequently, we defined the functional repertoire as the set of unique (excluding repetitions) patterns that happened over time and used its size as a marker of brain flexibility. Second, we used the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI)questionnaire to investigate the cohort's personality features. Finally, we investigated the relationship between personality dimensions, which was assessed by the TCI, and brain flexibility estimated as the size of the functional repertoire by a correlation analysis.

    Data from forty-six healthy subjects (27 females, 19 males, mean age: 27.6 ± 4.7 years) were included in the study. The dataset partially overlaps with the one presented by Troisi Lopez et al. [7]. The exclusion criteria included the following: age > 40 years, left-handedness, personal history of neurological, psychiatric, or psychological illness, and psychoactive drug use. All of the participants provided written informed consent. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Local Ethics Committee of the University of Naples “Federico II” (Prot. no. 17/2021).

    The participants completed the TCI consisting of 240 items true/false questionnaire [8]. TCI is a self-rated instrument that provides a comprehensive inventory of dimensions of temperament (Novelty Seeking, NS; Harm Avoidance, HA; Reward Dependence, RD; and Persistence, P) and character (Self-directedness, SD; Cooperativeness, C; and Self-transcendence, ST). In general, NS refers to the tendency to seek novel stimuli and experiences; HA refers to a tendency to inhibition of behavior in response to signals of punishment; RD refers to a tendency to the maintenance of behavior in response to cues of social reward; and P measures the tendency towards perseverance in the face of adversity. In sum, temperament refers to the set of responses to emotional stimuli: it is native, it manifests from birth, and it modifies throughout life. For the concerns the three domains of character, SD measures the ability to use the willpower necessary to achieve personal goals; C measures the ability to cooperate with others; and ST measures the ability to look beyond self-interest to see oneself as part of a larger whole, which could be described as a marker of maturity, but could also be described as a tendency towards spirituality or a belief in religious figures or the supernatural [7].

    The character is influenced both by sociocultural experiences and the path of individual maturation [8]. All these aspects of personality interact with each other, thus influencing the management of one's emotionality and behavioral choices.

    Table 1.  TCI assessment results of the sample in relation to each temperament and character.
    NS HA RD P S C ST
    Mean 0,460 0,440 0,649 0,652 0,717 0,807 0,393
    S.D. 0,164027 0,192991 0,167279 0,256733 0,142509 0,085748 0,181701
    Min 0,150 0,029 0,208 0,125 0,295 0,643 0,121
    Max 0,850 0,943 0,958 1,000 0,955 0,952 0,848

     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    For the acquisition of the magnetic resonance (MR) images, a 1.5-T scanner equipped with an 8-channel parallel coil head (General Electric Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI, USA) was utilized either after the MEG recording or a minimum of three weeks later (but not more than one month). Three-dimensional T1-weighted images (gradient-echo Inversion Recovery sequence prepared Fast Spoiled Gradient Recalled-echo, repetition time = 6988 ms, TI = 1100 ms, TE = 3.9 ms, flip angle = 10, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1.2 mm3) were acquired.

    MEG data collection took place within a system consisting of 163 magnetometers located in a magnetically shielded room (AtB Biomag UG, Ulm, Germany). Before data acquisition, the position of the four reference points (i.e., nasion, right and left pre-auricular, and apex) were digitized using the Fastrak system (Polhemus). The participants were instructed to maintain closed eyes and a relaxed state (resting-state condition) during two separate recording sessions, each lasting 3.5 minutes. The instructions were relayed via an intercom immediately preceding each recording session. After data acquisition, an anti-aliasing filter was applied, and sampling occurred at 1024 Hz. Subsequently, a smoothing filter was employed to eliminate components below 0.5 and above 48 Hz. This filtering process was conducted offline using MatLab scripts within the Fieldtrip 201456 toolbox. Concurrently, electrocardiogram (ECG) and electrooculogram (EOG) signals were also registered.

    To minimize the influence of environmental magnetic interference, we conducted a principal component analysis (PCA) using the resources provided by the Fieldtrip Toolbox on cerebral magnetic signals. This involved orthogonalizing the reference signals to establish a basis and then projecting the brain sensor signals onto this noise basis. Subsequently, we eliminated these projections to yield the purified data. An expert evaluator visually assessed the entire dataset, identifying and excluding noisy acquisition segments. Additionally, an independent component analysis (ICA) was employed to eliminate cardiac (usually 1–2 components) and ocular (0–1 components) artifacts from the raw brain signals. Several noisy recording segments were consequently discarded based on the inspection results, thus leading to the division of the recording into multiple trials. To ensure reliable connectivity estimates, only trials that exceeded 4 seconds in duration were considered for the analysis.

    We conducted a channel data source reconstruction by utilizing the beamforming technique integrated into the Fieldtrip toolbox [36]. Initially, subject fiducial points were employed to co-register the MEG data with the individual-specific MRI. Subsequently, by employing a single-shell volume conduction model and an equivalent current dipole source model, we applied a Linearly Constrained Minimum Variance (LCMV) beamformer to the entire pre-processed, wideband data [37]. This process aimed to reconstruct the time series data relative to the centroids of 116 regions of interest (ROIs), as delineated by the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas [38],[39]. Both the atlas and MRI were aligned with the head coordinates. We focused on the initial 90 ROIs and excluded those within the cerebellum due to the unreliable signal reconstruction in that region. A singular value decomposition (SVD) was utilized to project the time series along the direction of the dipole that explained the majority of the variance for each source. Subsequently, we visually assessed the source space data for each subject to ensure the absence of residual artifacts at the source level.

    The term “neuronal avalanche” denotes an occurrence characterized by substantial fluctuations in activity. To measure these fluctuations, we subjected each of the 90 reconstructed source signals to z-transformation, and subsequently applied a threshold of 3 standard deviations (i.e., z = 3) to each time series. Following this criterion, a neuronal avalanche commences when at least one region becomes active (i.e., surpasses the threshold of |z| > 3) and persists as long as any region remains above the threshold. It's worth noting that the spatial positions of the regions are not considered. Therefore, two coactive areas (temporally) are not necessarily adjacent and may also be situated in different and distant regions. However, the outcomes were not strictly influenced by the selection of this threshold. We conducted the analyses again, thereby setting the threshold at z = 2.75 and z = 3.25, and obtained similar results.

    These analyses necessitate binning the time series. To determine an appropriate bin length, we calculated the branching ratio [40], σ, as follows: for each time bin duration, for each subject, for each avalanche, the geometrically averaged ratio of the number of events (activations) between the subsequent time bin and the current time bin was computed as follows:

    i= 1Nbin-1j=1Nbin-1nevents j+1nevents j1Nbin-1

    where σi is the branching parameter of the i-th avalanche in the dataset, Nbin is the total number of bins in the i-th avalanche, and nevents(j) is the total number of events in the j-th bin. Then, we (geometrically) averaged the results over all avalanches [41] as follows:

    σ= 1Navali=1Navali1Naval

    where Naval is the total number of avalanches in each participant's dataset. In branching processes, a branching ratio of σ  = 1 indicates critical processes with activity that is highly variable and nearly sustained, σ < 1 indicates subcritical processes in which the activity quickly dies out, and σ > 1 indicates supercritical processes in which the activity increases. The bin length equal to three samples yielded a critical process with σ = 1, thus justifying the use of the term “avalanche” for these events. This means that each bin is obtained from three time-points of the binarized time-series. However, the robustness of the results to changes in this exact bin length was also investigated, and the main results were not affected. That is, different binnings yielded branching ratios very close to one, and the statistical differences in the exploration of the functional repertoire were similar for each binning. For each avalanche, an “avalanche configuration” was defined as the set of all areas that were above the threshold at some point during the avalanche. Note that the definition of avalanche configuration implies a loss of information in terms of the temporal structure within each avalanche. After identifying avalanches, the avalanche configurations were computed by an automated MATLAB script that recognizes which brain regions were involved in each avalanche and saves the specific configurations.

    We evaluated each participant's functional repertoire, which we define as the count of unique avalanche configurations expressed during the recording. By “unique”, we mean that each pattern of avalanche is tallied only once towards the total size of the functional repertoire. Indeed, since we are interested in assessing brain flexibility by means of the number of different configurations that the brain can present (i.e., the functional repertoire), even though many avalanches with the same configuration occur, we will count only one configuration, as it is identical across those avalanches. To ensure fairness in the comparisons among the participants, we considered the duration of the data acquisition, as it could influence the estimation of the functional repertoire. Consequently, the analysis was conducted using an equal amount of data for each participant (approximately 6 minutes). To achieve this, we measured the duration of each recording and identified the lowest one. Then, an automated script in MATLAB extracted a segment of data of the pre-identified lowest duration from each participant's recording, starting from a randomly selected time-point.

    Analyses were performed using MATLAB (MathWorks, version R2013a). The functional repertoire was correlated to the three TCI characters and four temperaments scores using Spearman's correlation. A significance level of 0.05 was applied after false discovery rate (FDR) correction across correlation with temperaments/characters.

    The brain critical state observation presupposes the calculation of the branching ratio of the time series binned at different lengths. For a threshold of z = 3, bin 1 results showed a branching ratio σ = 1. Hence, for the time series of bin 1, we calculated the number of visited patterns. Subsequently, a Spearman's correlation analysis was performed to estimate the relationship between the dynamic brain parameter and both the three dimensions of character and the four of temperament.

    As reported in Figure 1, the results show a statistically significant correlation between the number of visited patterns of configuration in broadband and the cooperativeness, which is one of the three TCI characters (r = 0.45, p = 0.002, pFDR = 0.012). We did not find further significant results in the correlation analysis. In addition, in order to check our results, the correlations were repeated by modifying the bins (bin = 2, pFDR = 0.005, r = 0.449; bin = 3, pFDR = 0.007, r = 0.434; bin = 4, pFDR = 0.007, r=0.434; bin = 5, pFDR = 0.009, r = 0.426) and the thresholds (thresholds 2.75, pFDR = 0.004 r = 0.458; thresholds 3.25, pFDR = 0.004, r = 0.456). The results confirmed the correlation.

    Figure 1.  Correlation between the number of avalanche patterns and TCI. The graph represents the Spearman's positive correlation between the number of visited patterns of configuration in broadband and the cooperativeness, one of the three TCI characters (r = 0.45, p = 0.002, pFDR = 0.012).

    In this preliminary study, we set out to investigate whether brain dynamics features may underlie specific personality dimensions. In particular, we recruited forty-six healthy individuals and investigated their brain dynamics activity by means of MEG. Then, we applied the theory of neuronal avalanches to identify activation patterns of brain regions and to measure brain flexibility. Subsequently, we investigated potential correlations between brain flexibility and the Cloninger model dimensions of personality, which revealed a significant positive correlation with the character of cooperativeness (C). This preliminary finding suggests that the higher an individual's reconfiguration of brain states, the higher the inclination for openness, tolerance, and, ideally, empathy toward others.

    By applying the neuronal avalanche theory, the analysis of brain flexibility used in this study overcame the limitations of previous studies that analyzed the biological correlates of personality dimensions without taking the continuous remodeling changes of the brain's functional connectome into account. In other words, our analysis of “functional repertoire” allowed us to obtain a dynamic measure of brain flexibility and, consequently, to study the personality characteristics with an innovative approach. In this case, we can interpret the data obtained by considering the dynamic nature of the psychological dimension of cooperativeness as the expression of a brain capable of effectively readjusting. As an example, we can report our previous MEG study conducted on a partially overlapping sample, in which we applied the network theory to evaluate the relation between the brain network topology and the personality dimensions [7]. In this study, we showed a negative correlation between the left caudate nucleus and the harm avoidance temperament, thus suggesting that this nucleus is involved in adaptive behavioral responses and in choosing actions that are likely to lead to a positive outcome [42]. Although the data obtained is interesting, the previous study did not allow us to evaluate the brain dynamics or analyze the relationship between continuous brain readjustments and the personality dimensions that are most affected by continuous situational changes. Indeed, brain flexibility encompasses the brain's ability to swiftly reconfigure itself in response to various environmental stimuli, thereby effectively encoding information. In social contexts, individuals engage with others, thereby communicating through verbal, paraverbal, and nonverbal means. The information involved in such interactions is highly complex, and effective communication between brain regions is essential to understand social cues, empathizing with others, and coordinating actions [43]. A flexible reconfiguration of brain patterns may enable the rapid integration of information from various sources, thus contributing to nuanced social responses. Additionally, considering that multiple individuals can participate in social exchanges and that an individual's attention is divided among various aspects (e.g., observing the environment, attending to physiological needs, situating oneself temporally), the cognitive workload becomes immensely substantial. In such instances, a high level of brain flexibility may allow one to effectively manage this multitasking task [44]. Consequently, an individual with a high degree of brain flexibility might find enjoyment in social interactions as they adeptly manage the situation and feel at ease. This propensity could correspondingly manifest as a high score in cooperativeness. Conversely, an individual with a low brain flexibility might struggle to rapidly process and manage the abundance of information present in socially charged situations, thus leading to difficulties and discomfort within the context [43]. Similarly, this might correlate with a low score in cooperativeness, which is in line with the aforementioned points.

    To our knowledge, few studies on the dynamics of the brain and personality dimensions have been conducted so far. Most of these studies have assessed personality using the five factor model (FFM), thereby showing that individual differences in conscientiousness traits are mediated by specific functional dynamics on a large scale [45] (Toschi et al. 201). Furthermore, Kabbara and colleagues [46] found that extraversion and openness are positively correlated with brain network dynamics. Similarly, Riccelli et al. showed that openness is associated with several brain regions, including parietal, temporal, and frontal areas, which are implicated in a wide range of sociocognitive functions [47]. This finding shows that the contribution of different regions from the whole brain network may be in line with our finding, which considers cooperativeness related to the reconfiguration of the brain system on a large scale. Cooperativeness could be associated with the trait of extroversion, as they share similar characteristics in social behaviors. In fact, a comparative study between FFM and TCI showed a relationship between the cooperativeness of TCI and the openness and extroversion of FFM [48]. From this point of view, greater cooperativeness and less egocentric and hostile behaviors can result from a flexible brain that modulates its activity by establishing flexible and adaptive interactions with its environment. It must be highlighted that the psychobiological model proposed by Cloninger takes the influence that learning and sociocultural factors exert on personality traits into great consideration, especially characters, since interactions between the human social system and the surrounding ecosystem strongly influence the latter. For this reason, it has been proposed to take the role of individual sociocultural experiences in mediating the relationship between brain dynamics and personality into consideration [29].

    In agreement with the dynamic approach to personality and taking the relation between brain flexibility and prosocial behaviors into account, personality and individual differences cannot be understood without understanding the dynamic processes of brain reconfiguration.

    Our preliminary study does not come without some limitations. First and foremost, it is important to note the limited size of our sample compared to traditional personality studies, which does not allow for intra-group analyses. However, it should be remembered that this is a preliminary study based on MEG data, and similar studies often report comparable sample sizes [46]. In this regard, it should be underlined that our sample is very homogeneous compared to other personality studies, thus allowing for a precise snapshot of personality dynamics in a specific age range. Another limitation is the exclusion of the cerebellum from the analysis of brain dynamics because our MEG system did not cover the lower occipital part and, therefore, did not allow a clean recording of the signal coming from the cerebellar nuclei. It is essential to note this limit in light of recent studies that reveal the cerebellar role in modulating individual differences in approach and avoidance behaviors by means of a cortico-basal-cerebellar loop [18],[19]. Furthermore, the choice to use the TCI instead of other personality questionnaires may also constitute a limit in the generalization of our results. Further studies, with a greater sample and a follow-up to verify the stability of the results, as well as the use of multimodal neuroimaging techniques or indicators to provide more comprehensive information about brain activity, will be necessary to confirm the relationship between personality dimensions and brain flexibility.

    Despite these limitations, the present preliminary study could be considered the first contribution to explain the neurobiological aspects of the dynamic system of personality, and it is part of an innovative line of research that underscores the potential of a network-based perspective to enhance understanding of brain-behavior relationships [49][51].


    Acknowledgments



    This work was supported by the European Union “Next Generation EU” (Investimento 3.1, M4, C2), Project IR0000011, EBRAINS-Italy of PNRR; ACCORDI PER INNOVAZIONE. Approccio User-friendly integrato per Diagnosi, Assistenza e Cura Efficaci - AUDACE. CUP: B69J23006050007

    Conflict of interest



    The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

    Availability of data



    The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

    Authors' contributions



    All authors designed the research. Francesco Ciaramella, Lorenzo Cipriano, Emahnuel Troisi Lopez, and Arianna Polverino performed the research. Emahnuel Troisi Lopez and Pierpaolo Sorrentino analyzed the data. Francesco Ciaramella, Lorenzo Cipriano, Pierpaolo Sorrentino and Laura Mandolesi wrote the paper. All authors read, revised, and approved the final manuscript.

    Ethics approval



    The study was approved by Ethical Committee of Psychological Research of the Department of Humanities of the University of Naples Federico II (n prot 11/2020) and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

    [1] Mandolesi L, Polverino A, Montuori S, et al. (2018) Effects of Physical Exercise on Cognitive Functioning and Wellbeing: Biological and Psychological Benefits. Front Psychol 9: 509. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00509
    [2] DeYoung CG, Gray JR (2009) Personality neuroscience: Explaining individual differences in affect, behaviour and cognition. Cambridge Handbook Pers Psychol : 323-346. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511596544.023
    [3] DeYoung CG (2010) Personality Neuroscience and the Biology of Traits: Personality Neuroscience. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 4: 1165-1180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
    [4] Markett S, Montag C, Reuter M (2018) Network neuroscience and personality. Pers Neurosci 1: e14. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2018.12
    [5] DeYoung CG, Beaty RE, Genç E, et al. (2022) Personality Neuroscience: An Emerging Field with Bright Prospects. Pers Sci 3: e7269. https://doi.org/10.5964/ps.7269
    [6] Sporns O, Tononi G, Kötter R (2005) The human connectome: A structural description of the human brain. PLOS Computat Biol 1: e42. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.0010042
    [7] Troisi Lopez E, Colonnello V, Liparoti M, et al. (2022) Brain network topology and personality traits: A source level magnetoencephalographic study. Scand J Psychol 63: 495-503. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12835
    [8] Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR (1993) A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiat 50: 975-990. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008
    [9] Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992) The five-factor model of personality and its relevance to personality disorders. J Pers Disord 6: 343-359. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.1992.6.4.343
    [10] Goldberg LR (1993) The structure of phenotypic personality traits. Am Psychol 48: 26-34. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.48.1.26
    [11] Omura K, Constable RT, Canli T (2005) Amygdala gray matter concentration is associated with extraversion and neuroticism. Neuroreport 16: 1905-1908. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000186596.64458.76
    [12] Blankstein U, Chen JY, Mincic AM, et al. (2009) The complex minds of teenagers: neuroanatomy of personality differs between sexes. Neuropsychologia 47: 599-603. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.014
    [13] Coutinho JF, Sampaio A, Ferreira M, et al. (2013) Brain correlates of pro-social personality traits: a voxel-based morphometry study. Brain Imaging Behav 7: 293-299. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-013-9227-2
    [14] Liu WY, Weber B, Reuter M, et al. (2013) The Big Five of Personality and structural imaging revisited: a VBM–DAR℡ study. Neuroreport 24: 375-380. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e328360dad7
    [15] Taki Y, Thyreau B, Kinomura S, et al. (2013) A longitudinal study of the relationship between personality traits and the annual rate of volume changes in regional gray matter in healthy adults. Hum Brain Mapp 34: 3347-53. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22145
    [16] Lu F, Huo Y, Li M, et al. (2014) Relationship between personality and gray matter volume in healthy young adults: a voxel-based morphometric study. PLoSOne 9: e88763. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088763
    [17] Aghajani M, Veer IM, van Tol MJ, et al. (2014) Neuroticism and extraversion are associated with amygdala resting-state functional connectivity. Cogn Affect Behav Ne 14: 836-48. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0224-0
    [18] Picerni E, Petrosini L, Piras F, et al. (2013) New evidence for the cerebellar involvement in personality traits. Front Behav Neurosci 7: 133. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00133
    [19] Laricchiuta D, Petrosini L, Piras F, et al. (2014) Linking novelty seeking and harm avoidance personality traits to cerebellar volumes. Hum Brain Mapp 35: 285-296. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22174
    [20] Petrosini L, Cutuli D, Picerni E, et al. (2017) Viewing the Personality Traits Through a Cerebellar Lens: a Focus on the Constructs of Novelty Seeking, Harm Avoidance, and Alexithymia. Cerebellum 16: 178-190. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-015-0754-9
    [21] Clausi S, Siciliano L, Olivito G, et al. (2022) Cerebellum and Emotion in Social Behavior. Adv Exp Med Biol 1378: 235-253. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-99550-8_15
    [22] Terburg D, van Honk J, Schutter DJLG (2024) Doubling down on dual systems: A cerebellum-amygdala route towards action- and outcome-based social and affective behavior. Cortex 173: 175-186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2024.02.002
    [23] Chialvo DR (2010) Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nat Phys 6: 744-750. https://doi.org/10.1038/nphys1803
    [24] Shriki O, Alstott J, Carver F, et al. (2013) Neuronal avalanches in the resting MEG of the human brain. J Neurosci 33: 7079-90. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4286-12.2013
    [25] Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR (2011) Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 12: 43-56. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2961
    [26] Tagliazucchi E, Carhart-Harris R, Leech R, et al. (2014) Enhanced repertoire of brain dynamical states during the psychedelic experience. Hum Brain Mapp 35: 5442-5456. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22562
    [27] Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, et al. (2011) The importance of being variable. J Neurosci 31: 4496-4503. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5641-10.2011
    [28] Hutchison RM, Womelsdorf T, Allen EA, et al. (2013) Dynamic functional connectivity: promise, issues, and interpretations. Neuroimage 80: 360-78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079
    [29] Medaglia JD, Lynall ME, Bassett DS (2015) Cognitive network neuroscience. J Cogn Neurosci 27: 1471-1491. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00810
    [30] Sorrentino P, Seguin C, Rucco R, et al. (2021) The structural connectome constrains fast brain dynamics. Elife 10: e67400. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.67400
    [31] Bassett DS, Wymbs NF, Porter MA, et al. (2011) Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain networks during learning. P Natl Acad Sci 108: 7641-7646. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1018985108
    [32] Braun U, Schäfer A, Walter H, et al. (2015) Dynamic reconfiguration of frontal brain networks during executive cognition in humans. P Natl Acad Sci 112: 11678-83. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1422487112
    [33] Gerraty RT, Davidow JY, Foerde K, et al. (2018) Dynamic flexibility in striatal-cortical circuits supports reinforcement learning. J Neurosci 38: 2442-2453. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2084-17.2018
    [34] Paban V, Modolo J, Mheich A, et al. (2019) Psychological resilience correlates with EEG source-space brain network flexibility. Netw Neurosci 3: 539-550. https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00079
    [35] Quirin M, Malekzad F, Paudel D, et al. (2023) Dynamics of personality: The Zurich model of motivation revived, extended, and applied to personality. J Pers 91: 928-946. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12805
    [36] Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, et al. (2011) FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intel Neurosci 2011: 156869. https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/156869
    [37] Van Veen BD, van Drongelen W, Yuchtman M, et al. (1997) Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE T Biomed Eng 44: 867-880. https://doi.org/10.1109/10.623056
    [38] Gong G, He Y, Concha L, et al. (2009) Mapping anatomical connectivity patterns of human cerebral cortex using in vivo diffusion tensor imaging tractography. Cereb Cortex 19: 524-36. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn102
    [39] Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, et al. (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15: 273-89. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978
    [40] Haldeman C, Beggs JM (2005) Critical branching captures activity in living neural networks and maximizes the number of metastable States. Phys Rev J 94: 058101. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.94.058101
    [41] Sorrentino P, Rucco R, Baselice F, et al. (2021) Flexible brain dynamics underpins complex behaviours as observed in Parkinson's disease. Sci Rep 11: 4051. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-83425-4
    [42] Tricomi EM, Delgado MR, Fiez JA (2004) Modulation of caudate activity by action contingency. Neuron 41: 281-292. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00848-1
    [43] Swanson HL, Malone S (1992) Social Skills and Learning Disabilities: A Meta-Analysis of the Literature. Psychol Rev 21: 427-443. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.1992.12085627
    [44] Boyd R, Richerson PJ, Henrich J (2011) The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. P Natl Acad Sci 108: 10918-10925. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100290108
    [45] Toschi N, Riccelli R, Indovina I, et al. (2018) Functional connectome of the five-factor model of personality. Pers Neurosci 1: e2. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2017.2
    [46] Kabbara A, Paban V, Weill A, et al. (2020) Brain Network Dynamics Correlate with Personality Traits. Brain Connect 10: 108-120. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2019.0723
    [47] Riccelli R, Toschi N, Nigro S, et al. (2017) Surface-based morphometry reveals the neuroanatomical basis of the five-factor model of personality. Soc Cogn Affect Neur 12: 671-684. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw175
    [48] De Fruyt F, Van de Wiele L, Van Heeringen C (2000) Cloninger's psychobiological model of temperament and character and the five-factor model of personality. Pers Indiv Differ 29: 441-452. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00204-4
    [49] Cai H, Zhu J, Yu Y (2020) Robust prediction of individual personality from brain functional connectome. Soc Cogn Affect Neur 15: 359-369. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa044
    [50] Zhang X, Xu R, Ma H, et al. (2024) Brain Structural and Functional Damage Network Localization of Suicide. Biol Psychiat 95: 1091-1099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2024.01.003
    [51] Cheng Y, Cai H, Liu S, et al. (2024) Brain Network Localization of Gray Matter Atrophy and Neurocognitive and Social Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2024.07.021
  • Reader Comments
  • © 2024 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(662) PDF downloads(41) Cited by(0)

Figures and Tables

Figures(1)  /  Tables(1)

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog