Citation: Alexis M. Chambers, Jessica D. Payne. The Influence of Sleep on the Consolidation of Positive Emotional Memories: Preliminary Evidence[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2014, 1(1): 39-51. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.1.39
[1] | Marie Vandekerckhove, Yu-lin Wang . Emotion, emotion regulation and sleep: An intimate relationship. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(1): 1-17. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.1.1 |
[2] | Monireh Asadi Ghaleni, Forouzan Fattahi Masrour, Narjes Saryar, Alexandra J. Bratty, Ebrahim Norouzi, Matheus Santos de Sousa Fernandes, Georgian Badicu . Effects of an intervention combining physical activity and components of Amygdala and Insula Retraining (AIR) on sleep and working memory among older male adults. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(4): 421-438. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024025 |
[3] | Antonio G. Lentoor . Cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying false memories: misinformation, distortion or erroneous configuration?. AIMS Neuroscience, 2023, 10(3): 255-268. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2023020 |
[4] | Ritwik Das, Artur Luczak . Epileptic seizures and link to memory processes. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(1): 114-127. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022007 |
[5] | Timothy J. Ricker . The Role of Short-term Consolidation in Memory Persistence. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(4): 259-279. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.4.259 |
[6] | Andrea J. Rapkin, Steven M. Berman, Edythe D. London . The Cerebellum and Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder. AIMS Neuroscience, 2014, 1(2): 120-141. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.2.120 |
[7] | Pauline M. Insch, Gillian Slessor, Louise H. Phillips, Anthony Atkinson, Jill Warrington . The Impact of Aging and Alzheimers Disease on Decoding Emotion Cues from Bodily Motion. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(3): 139-152. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.3.139 |
[8] | Galina V. Portnova, Michael S. Atanov . Nonlinear EEG parameters of emotional perception in patients with moderate traumatic brain injury, coma, stroke and schizophrenia. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(4): 221-235. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.4.221 |
[9] | Wanda M. Snow, Brenda M. Stoesz, Judy E. Anderson . The Cerebellum in Emotional Processing: Evidence from Human and Non-Human Animals. AIMS Neuroscience, 2014, 1(1): 96-119. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.1.96 |
[10] | Justyna Gerłowska, Krzysztof Dmitruk, Konrad Rejdak . Facial emotion mimicry in older adults with and without cognitive impairments due to Alzheimer's disease. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(2): 226-238. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021012 |
Extensive evidence demonstrates that sleep aids the consolidation of emotionally negative vs. neutral memory [1,2,3]. In the context of this literature, ‘negative memory’ is defined as memory performance on tests of previously encoded negative materials, such as narrative passages that contain descriptions of murder [1] or pictures depicting gory scenes (e.g., a severed hand; [2,4]). Sleep as opposed to wakefulness leads to greater memory enhancements of such negative materials compared to neutral counterparts both when this material is learned prior to nocturnal sleep [1,2], as well as prior to a daytime nap [3]. Such effects may be related to the amount of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep obtained during the sleep interval [1,3,5], a stage associated with increases in activity of emotional memory regions (e.g., amygdala, anterior cingulate, hippocampus) as well as changes in neurochemicals related to emotional processing (e.g., acetylcholine, norepinephrine, cortisol) [6,7,8,9].
Nishida and colleagues [3] found that the amount of REM sleep and corresponding theta electroencephalography (EEG) rhythms, as well as how quickly participants first entered REM sleep (i.e., REM sleep latency) during a post-encoding nap was related to memory improvements of negative pictures, which benefitted from increased recognition rates by those who napped compared to those who remained awake during the consolidation delay. Moreover, in a study by Wagner, Gais, and Born [1], participants read and memorized either negative or neutral narrative descriptions at encoding, and free recall was tested after the first half of a night of sleep, which contains proportionately more non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM) than REM sleep, the second half of nocturnal sleep, which contains more REM than NREM sleep, or comparable periods of wakefulness. They found that those who slept, and particularly those who obtained the REM-rich sleep late in the night, recalled more of the negative descriptions than neutral descriptions. Additionally, a follow-up four years later revealed that both sleep groups had comparable memory for these emotional writings, likely because the early sleep group was eventually allowed the late-night, REM-rich portion of sleep after initial testing [10]. Thus, many studies have corroborated the finding that REM is related to improved emotional memory [1,3,10].
However, sleep’s ability to preserve emotional memories has been shown to operate with a precision that extends beyond entire pictures or passages. Payne and colleagues [4] presented participants with scenes that consisted of either a negative or neutral foreground object placed on a neutral background at encoding. Following a period of nocturnal sleep or daytime wakefulness, participants were tested on the foreground objects and backgrounds, presented separately and intermixed with new images. They found that sleep exaggerated an “emotional memory trade-off” over wakefulness, whereby negative foreground objects were remembered better than neutral ones, but at the expense of the memory for their corresponding neutral backgrounds, which were not remembered as well as those originally paired with neutral objects [4]. Critically, sleep had its greatest effects on the most negative components of the scenes, with memory for the negative objects improved after sleep not only above that seen after an equivalent period of wakefulness, but also after just a 30-minute waking delay. Further study has shown that this preferential enhancement of memory for negative objects following sleep is positively correlated with amounts of REM sleep achieved during the night [5], corroborating previous research [3], and pointing toward a possible physiological mechanism for this sleep-dependent memory enhancement [5].
Although this body of literature provides support for sleep’s impact on emotional memory formation, and suggests possible biological mechanisms active during REM sleep, one problem with these studies is the lack of information they provide about how sleep affects positive memory, such as memory for positive images. Yet such a question is important to address because it can inform research in applied domains, such as the treatment of mood disorders. For example, Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) results in a bias to remember or ruminate on negative information, resulting in many MDD studies that focus on negative emotions. However, studies by Heller et al. [11] and Gruber et al. [12] highlight the importance of positive emotion, finding those with MDD to experience positive emotions less and have a smaller neural response to positive stimuli than healthy individuals. Coupled with the fact that MDD is often related to abnormalities in sleep architecture, including increased REM sleep amount and decreased REM sleep latency [13], information gleaned from research on sleep’s impact on memory for positive information could play a role in treatment.
Broadly speaking, positive emotion does aid memory, with research revealing that positive emotion enhances memory for stimuli such as video clips [14] and pictures [15,16]. This is likely due to the fact that positive information, like negative information, activates the amygdala [17,18], with this activation modulating activity in the hippocampus for processing of both positive and negative information [15]. Further, there is some evidence that memory trade-offs do occur for positive information when positive images are added to the emotional memory trade-off task [19,20], though this effect may not be as robust as that for negative images. Previous research has shown the emotional memory trade-off to occur for positive stimuli only at certain levels of arousal [19,20], or to disappear at longer delays, after which memory for positive objects remains better than that for neutral objects, but the backgrounds paired with positive objects are no longer impaired relative to those paired with neutral objects [19].
As an initial investigation of the specific and neglected influence of sleep on positive memory, the current study examined the evolution of the emotional memory trade-off for positive information across periods of nocturnal sleep vs. daytime wakefulness. In order to imitate previous sleep and emotional memory trade-off studies, which compared negative to neutral scene memory [4,5,21], positive scenes were compared only to neutral scenes in the current experiment.
If sleep acts on positive and negative information in a similar manner, as some theories of emotional memory would suggest [17,18], then sleep-based consolidation processes should not only preferentially enhance memory for positive objects as opposed to neutral objects in the current study, but should also lead to a corresponding deficit in memory for positive backgrounds compared to neutral ones, over and above that seen during wakefulness [4,5,21,22]. Alternatively, if the trade-off effect is truly not as robust for positive information as it is for negative information, sleep may only selectively preserve memory for positive objects, without a concurrent decrement to their backgrounds [19,20]. Regardless, given the similarities between the processing of positive and negative memories [15,17,18], and the neurobiology of REM sleep that makes it ideal for emotional processing [6,7,8,9], memory for positive (but not neutral) aspects of the stimuli should positively correlate with markers of REM sleep (i.e., time spent in REM sleep, REM sleep latency), similar to results of previous trade-off studies that found a positive correlation between REM sleep and memory for negative scene components [3,5].
Participants were Notre Dame students (N = 93) who received either course credit or payment for participation. Of these participants, 29 constituted the Wake group, 31 the Sleep group, 16 the AM control group, and 17 the PM control group (see below for details about control groups). Participants were between the ages of 18 and 32 (M = 19.59 ± 1.82) and were 52% female. In order to participate, participants had to indicate that they were fluent in English, had normal or corrected vision, and were not taking any medications that affected the central nervous system, nor had a history of psychiatric or sleep disorders. This study was approved by the University of Notre Dame Institutional Review Board. All participants provided written informed consent.
Picture stimuli. Trade-off task stimuli consisted of pictures taken from Payne et al. [4] and Waring and Kensinger [19] who have previously investigated the emotional memory trade-off. The set included a positive (e.g., chocolate cake) or neutral (e.g., light bulb) object placed on a neutral background (e.g., kitchen setting or nightstand, respectively; Figure 1). Positive and neutral objects were selected based on previous ratings of arousal and valence from 23 Notre Dame undergraduates. Ratings of valence were made using a scale of one (positive) to five (negative) with neutral in the center. Ratings of arousal were also made using a scale of one (low) to five (high). Selected positive objects had a mean valence of 1.86 ± 0.21 and a mean arousal of 3.12 ± 0.28. Neutral objects had a mean valence of 2.90 ± 1.00 and a mean arousal of 1.26 ± 0.09. An independent t-test revealed that positive objects were rated significantly lower on valence (i.e., more positive) than neutral objects, t(62) = -25.06, p < 0.001. Further, positive items were significantly more arousing than neutral items t(62) = 36.13, p < 0.001.
Subjects participated in two sessions, separated by a 12-hour delay. Those in the Wake group encoded the scenes at approximately 9am and were tested at 9pm, while those in the Sleep group encoded the scenes at approximately 9pm and were tested at 9am. After encoding, those in the Wake group were allowed to leave the lab, while Sleep subjects were wired for sleep-recording polysomnography (PSG) and spent the night in the laboratory. This allowed the analysis of standard sleep staging (NREM stages N1-N3, REM). To control for time-of-day confounds, two additional groups were tested, encoding the material at either 9am (AM control) or 9pm (PM control) and being tested 20 minutes later [22]. Comparison of the two control groups was used to exclude time-of-day interpretations.
At the beginning of session one, participants in the Sleep and Wake groups completed a series of questionnaires assessing demographics, recent sleep habits, mood, anxiety, depression, and personality characteristics (Table 1). Participants then completed the emotional memory trade-off encoding task [4,19,22], which required the incidental encoding of 64 scenes displaying 32 high-arousal positive and 32 low-arousal neutral foreground objects (e.g., a cake or light bulb, respectively) on neutral backgrounds (e.g., a kitchen counter or nightstand, respectively; Figure 1). Participants studied the scenes for 5 seconds each and then rated the valence and arousal of each scene. Valence was rated on a scale of one (positive) to seven (negative) with neutral in the center of the scale. Arousal was also rated on a scale of one (calm) to seven (excited). Pictures were presented on a Windows system using E-Prime 2.0 software.
Measure | Sleep | Wake | t-value | p-value |
All comparisons were non-significant. | ||||
Negative Affect | 13.58 | 13.39 | 0.17 | 0.86 |
Positive Affect | 24.61 | 22.71 | 1.05 | 0.30 |
STAI | 38.61 | 37.68 | 0.44 | 0.66 |
PSQI | 4.68 | 4.26 | 0.81 | 0.42 |
BDI | 4.59 | 3.81 | 0.84 | 0.40 |
Following this task, those in the Wake group were allowed to leave the lab, but were asked to refrain from napping during the day. Alternatively, those in the Sleep group were immediately wired for PSG recording while watching a non-arousing video. This occurred between 10pm and 11pm. All subjects were given an 8.5-hour sleep opportunity, with lights-out between 11:15pm and 11:30pm. Sleep was recorded with Grass polysomnography systems. A montage using EOG (electrooculography), EMG (electromyography) and EEG leads placed at F3, F4, C3, Cz, C4, O1, and O2 scalp positions was used, with each electrode referenced to the contralateral mastoid. Sleep data were scored according to the American Academy of Sleep Medicine [23] standards.
During session two, the Wake and Sleep groups began by completing brief questionnaires to assess napping, caffeine use, and perceived sleep time during the 12-hr delay. Remember/Know recognition memory [24,25] was then tested by presenting the 128 old (previously seen) and 64 new (foils) objects and backgrounds separately and one at a time, and asking participants which were seen at encoding. Those in the Sleep group began the task approximately 30 minutes after lights-on to prevent effects of sleep inertia on memory performance. Participants were asked to respond with one of three responses: Remember indicated that they vividly remembered seeing the image at encoding along with the context it was presented in, Know indicated that the image seemed familiar to them but they could not remember the exact context in which it was viewed at encoding, and New indicated that they did not see the image at encoding. This Remember/Know paradigm allows the investigation of both vivid memory and familiarity, similar to previous studies [2].
Those in the AM and PM control groups completed all tasks described above, but in a different order to accommodate the short time delay between encoding and testing. Thus, the control groups began with the trade-off encoding task and then completed the session-one questionnaires described above. The questionnaires prevented rehearsal during the 20-minute delay, which was spent in the lab under supervision of the experimenter. The control groups did not complete the session-two questionnaires, which were primarily used to inquire of the wake group’s activities during the delay outside the lab. Following completion of the questionnaires, controls performed the Remember/Know recognition test following the same instructions given to the Sleep and Wake groups.
Because of the independence of Remember and Know responses, direct proportions of Remember responses were used to indicate recollection (R) while a correction was applied to Know responses (K/1-R) to prevent underestimation of the familiarity measure [24,25]. Further, hits (i.e., correctly identifying a previously seen item as old) were used in conjunction with rates of false alarms (i.e., incorrectly identifying a new item as old) to yield a corrected memory measure (i.e., hits — false alarms), even though the rate of false alarms was low (less than 2% for Remember responses, and less than 13% for Know responses) and was not significantly different between the groups (all p’s > 0.11).
Further, given that the trade-off effect for negative information appears to be driven by sleep’s selective preservation of negative objects [4], as well as previous research suggesting that the trade-off effect may not be as robust for positive information as it is for negative information [19,20], we conducted two a priori analyses of the data. First, we investigated the full trade-off effect between the groups with a 2 (Valence: positive, neutral) by 2 (Component: object, background) by 2 (Group: Sleep, Wake) mixed ANOVA for both Remember and Know responses. Second, we used t-tests to directly compare memory for objects and backgrounds between the groups to investigate whether sleep selectively benefited the positive components of scenes (i.e. the objects).
Sleep vs. wake groups. We first investigated whether the Sleep and Wake groups were comparable on baseline measures of affect, anxiety, sleep quality, and depression measures. Independent t-tests indicated that the groups did not differ on any baseline measures (Table 1).
Am vs. pm controls. Independent t-tests were also conducted to investigate baseline differences between the AM and PM control groups on measures of affect, anxiety, sleep quality, and depression. No significant differences were found on any of the measures (Table 2).
Measure | AM | PM | t-value | p-value |
All comparisons were non-significant. | ||||
Negative Affect | 14.75 | 13.29 | 0.72 | 0.48 |
Positive Affect | 24.81 | 25.94 | -0.43 | 0.67 |
STAI | 41.37 | 37.41 | 1.31 | 0.20 |
PSQI | 4.63 | 4.88 | -0.28 | 0.78 |
BDI | 6.75 | 4.47 | 1.26 | 0.22 |
Given prior evidence suggesting that the full trade-off effect is not always obtained with positive (as opposed to negative) scenes, we first examined whether the emotional memory trade-off occurred for positive stimuli generally (i.e., across both the Sleep and Wake groups). An ANOVA on Remember responses revealed main effects of valence, F(1, 58) = 91.22, p < 0.001, and component, F(1.58) = 15.63, p < 0.001, which were qualified by a valence by component interaction, F(1, 58) = 180.75, p < 0.001. Although positive objects (M = 0.63 ± 0.22) were remembered better than neutral objects (M = 0.33 ± 0.20), t(59) = 15.32, p < 0.001, their associated backgrounds (M = 0.41 ± 0.19) were equally well remembered compared to the backgrounds associated with neutral objects (M = 0.43 ± 0.18), t(59) = -1.37, p > 0.17, a finding inconsistent with the full emotional memory trade-off effect. A similar analysis on Know responses revealed main effects of valence, F(1, 58) = 17.91, p = 0.001, and component, F(1, 58) = 7.41, p = 0.009, which were qualified by a valence by component interaction, F(1, 58) = 46.34, p < 0.001. In this case, not only were positive objects (M = 0.59 ± 0.28) remembered better than neutral ones (M = 0.37 ± 0.22), t(59) = 6.98, p < 0.001, but backgrounds associated with positive objects (M = 0.38 ± 0.20) were also remembered worse than those associated with neutral objects (M = 0.44 ± 0.20), t(59) = -2.69, p < 0.01, consistent with the full emotional memory trade-off effect.
Upon examining whether the trade-off effect differed across the Sleep and Wake groups, we obtained a main effect of group on recollection scores, F(1, 58) = 4.43, p = 0.04, with the Sleep group (M = 0.49, SEM = 0.03) performing better than the Wake group (M = 0.40, SEM = 0.03) overall. However, there were no interactions between measures of memory and group (all p’s > 0.45). Specifically, the lack of a three-way interaction among the variables F(1, 58) = 0.046, p > 0.80, indicates that there were no differences in the trade-off effect across groups, a finding that may be driven by the absence of a full trade-off effect in the recollection of emotionally positive stimuli. There was no group main effect, F(1, 58) = 0.27, p > 0.60, and no interactions with group (all p’s > 0.20) for scores of familiarity.
However, per our a priori hypothesis, we probed the data further by specifically comparing memory for objects and backgrounds between the Sleep and Wake groups. Here, as predicted, we found evidence that the Sleep group recollected positive objects better than the Wake group, t(58) = 2.09, p = 0.04 (Figure 2). Importantly, this was the only significant effect; the Sleep and Wake groups did not differ on memory for neutral objects, backgrounds paired with neutral objects, or backgrounds paired with positive objects (see Figure 2 and Table 3). Thus, partially consistent with prior studies examining memory for emotionally negative information within scenes [4,5], sleep influenced memory for the emotional, and in this case positive, components of the scenes. For familiarity, there were no differences between the groups for any of the scene components (Table 4).
Sleep | Wake | t-value | |||
* = p < 0.05 | |||||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
Positive Objects | 0.69 | 0.21 | 0.58 | 0.21 | 2.09 * |
Neutral Objects | 0.37 | 0.18 | 0.29 | 0.21 | 1.77 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.45 | 0.21 | 0.36 | 0.17 | 1.82 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.47 | 0.17 | 0.39 | 0.19 | 1.59 |
Sleep | Wake | t-value | |||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
All comparisons were non-significant | |||||
Positive Objects | 0.58 | 0.31 | 0.61 | 0.25 | -0.34 |
Neutral Objects | 0.37 | 0.23 | 0.36 | 0.22 | 0.13 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.40 | 0.24 | 0.35 | 0.15 | 1.01 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.47 | 0.21 | 0.41 | 0.18 | 1.10 |
Pearson’s r correlations were used to investigate the relationship between emotional memory performance and REM sleep features. Although there was no relationship between recollection of positive objects and total time spent in REM sleep (r = -0.10, p > 0.60), or REM latency (r = 0.10, p > 0.50), there was a significant correlation between familiarity for positive objects and REM latency (r = -0.43, p = 0.017; Figure 3). The more quickly participants entered REM sleep, the better was their memory for positive objects, a finding consistent with our hypothesis of a role for REM sleep in positive memory formation. There was no relationship between any other memory and sleep measures.
To ensure there were no time-of-day effects on memory performance, a 2 (Control group: AM, PM) by 2 (Valence: positive, neutral) by 2 (Component: object, background) mixed ANOVA was performed on the control groups’ Remember responses. Results revealed no significant interactions with group, and no group main effect, F(1, 31) = 1.32, p > 0.25. Further, there were no differences between the groups for recollection memory of any of the scene components (Table 5). Similarly, for Know responses, there were no significant interactions with group, and no group main effect, F(1, 31) = 0.02, p > 0.90. Finally, there were no differences between the groups for familiarity of any of the scene components (Table 6).
AM Control | PM Control | t-value | |||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
All comparisons were non-significant | |||||
Positive Objects | 0.83 | 0.17 | 0.85 | 0.15 | -0.38 |
Neutral Objects | 0.51 | 0.23 | 0.58 | 0.23 | -0.80 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.58 | 0.16 | 0.67 | 0.22 | -1.28 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.55 | 0.16 | 0.64 | 0.18 | -1.44 |
AM Control | PM Control | t-value | |||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
All comparisons were non-significant | |||||
Positive Objects | 0.59 | 0.36 | 0.45 | 0.38 | 1.02 |
Neutral Objects | 0.39 | 0.24 | 0.43 | 0.26 | -0.42 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.33 | 0.14 | 0.35 | 0.26 | -0.26 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.38 | 0.21 | 0.42 | 0.25 | -0.51 |
The current study investigated the existence of the emotional memory trade-off effect for positive compared to neutral images after a period of nocturnal sleep and daytime wakefulness. While there was a general enhancement of memory for positive objects over neutral ones, consistent with the emotional memory trade-off, the corresponding decrement to backgrounds associated with positive objects as compared to neutral ones occurred only for memory measures of familiarity, and was not impacted differently by a period of sleep or wakefulness. Yet, for measures of recollection, we found that sleep enhanced memory over wakefulness for positive object memory specifically, with no differences between the groups on memory for any of the neutral components of the studied scenes. This is the first study to provide preliminary evidence that sleep enhances recollection for specifically positive components of memories.
The emotional memory trade-off has been indicated in previous research not only by better memory for emotional objects as compared neutral objects, but also by a corresponding decrement in memory for backgrounds associated with emotional objects compared to those associated with neutral objects [4,19,20]. In the present study, there is some limited evidence for this trade-off effect for positive scenes, but only for measures of familiarity. The positive memory trade-off for measures of recollection was not obtained due to the similarity of memory for backgrounds paired with positive and neutral objects. These findings are inconsistent with previous demonstrations of the emotional memory trade-off effect for negative scenes, which obtained the trade-off across all measures of memory specificity [4]. It may be possible that the trade-off effect is less robust for positive information. Indeed, the few studies that have investigated the emotional memory trade-off for positive information only obtained the positive memory trade-off at certain levels of arousal [20], or have found that the decrement in memory for backgrounds placed with positive compared to neutral objects disappeared over longer delays [19]. Thus, it is possible that consolidation processes impact memory for the components of positive scenes differently than negative scenes.
Alternatively, the lack of the full trade-off effect for recollection memory in this study may be because of the instructions given for the Remember/Know test. Specifically, Remember responses required participants to retrieve contextual details, while for Know responses, such contextual details were not able to be retrieved. It is possible that the details required for Remember responses prevented the trade-off effect for recollection because background memory would be necessary for such responses. Thus, the methods used to test memory may have important consequences for obtaining the trade-off effect.
Investigation of the emotional memory trade-off effect between the Sleep and Wake groups revealed that participants in the Sleep group had better recollection for scene components overall compared to the Wake group, while familiarity was comparable across both groups. This suggests that sleep impacted the ability for participants to remember more detailed, vivid memories, as opposed to information they were simply familiar with. Further, examining memory for object and background components separately highlighted differences in recollection memory between the groups. Crucially, although there were no differences in memory for backgrounds between groups, or in memory for neutral objects, memory for specifically positive objects was enhanced in the Sleep group relative to the Wake group. Since this memory enhancement was specific to the emotional components, the current study does offer partial support for an active role of sleep on the consolidation of positive emotional material. This is in line with previous study of sleep’s impact on negative memory, which found sleep to have its strongest effect on negative information, enhancing memory for negative objects above neutral objects compared to a period of wakefulness [4]. Such similarities suggest that there may be common sleep-dependent processes occurring for positive and negative information. However, because the results of the present study do not map perfectly onto findings using negative stimuli, which have found sleep to impact the full trade-off effect for negative information over a period of wakefulness [4,5], future work will be necessary to fully delineate the nature of any differences between the sleep dependent processing of positive and negative memories.
It is also important to note that the sample used in the current study was drawn from a university student population, but that investigating older populations may also yield interesting results for positive memory formation. The sample for the current study was chosen because it has been the focus of previous research in this area [4], and thus allowed us to make comparisons to previous findings. For this reason, the age range we used was an ideal starting point for investigations into the primary question of this study. However, other positive memory studies have examined older adult populations, which is important given that older individuals have been suggested to show differences in emotional memory performance compared to younger adults, even showing a greater preference for positive compared to negative information [19]. Thus, future work in this area will benefit from investigations into varying age groups.
Although the Sleep and Wake groups performed the encoding and testing sessions at different times of the day, there is little concern about time-of-day effects on the current results given that there were no differences found in the performance of the AM and PM control groups. Despite completing all experimental tasks at drastically different times of the day, these groups showed no differences on any of the memory measures in the current study. Therefore, the differences between the Sleep and Wake groups were not due to time-of-day influences, but were a result of the period of sleep or wake obtained between encoding and testing.
Measures of REM sleep are commonly associated with emotional memory performance [1,3,5]. This is likely because emotional centers of the brain that support the encoding and retrieval of emotional memories (e.g., the amygdala; [17]) are intensely activated during REM sleep [6], and dramatic increases in neurochemicals associated with emotional learning, such as acetylcholine, norepinephrine, and cortisol, are observed during this stage [6,7,8,9]. While previous studies have found evidence of this association for negative information [1,3,5], the current study obtained evidence for a relationship between positive memory performance and REM latency. The faster one achieved the first stage of REM sleep, the better their familiarity memory of positive objects, thus providing initial support for a connection between REM sleep and positive memory consolidation. Interestingly, a similar relationship between REM latency and memory was recently reported, but for negative pictures [3]. Such a relationship may indicate that quicker entry into REM results in more intense emotional processing, or may be the result of a greater need for such processing, regardless of memory valence. Given that the unique neurobiology of this state likely supports processing of emotional information [6,8,9], achieving such a state more quickly might reflect better and more efficient processing of the information, which may be adaptive for the individual. Future research will be needed to determine how this earlier processing benefits emotional memory enhancements.
It should also be noted that while amount of REM sleep has also been associated with memory performance for negative materials [3,5], REM sleep amount did not correlate with memory for positive information in the present study. Although the reason for this lack of association is presently unclear, one possibility is that the REM sleep — emotional memory connection is stronger for negative than for positive memory. Follow-up investigations are currently underway to test this idea.
In summary, the current study investigated sleep’s influence on the neglected emotional memory trade-off effect for positive information. Recollection memory specifically for the positive components of images was enhanced by sleep, partially extending previous findings for memory of negative material. However, while the trade-off effect for negative scenes exists across varying levels of memory specificity, the trade-off effect for positive scenes was eliminated for the most detailed memories. Although future research is needed to fully understand what leads to these differences between memory for positive and negative information after a period of sleep-dependent consolidation, this work offers a crucial and necessary first step to expand our knowledge of the complex relationship between sleep, positive emotion, and memory.
The authors wish to thank Michelle Wirth and G.A. Radvansky for their advice on the design of this study, as well as Elizabeth Kensinger and Jill Waring for providing us with the picture stimuli. We also would like to thank Sara Alger and Tony Cunningham for their help editing the final manuscript.
All authors declare no conflicts of interest in this paper.
[1] |
Wagner U, Gais S, Born J. (2001) Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learn Memory 8: 112-119. doi: 10.1101/lm.36801
![]() |
[2] |
Hu P, Stylos-Allan M, Walker MP. (2006) Sleep facilitates consolidation of emotional declarative memory. Psychol Sci 17: 891-898. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01799.x
![]() |
[3] |
Nishida M, Pearsall J, Buckner RL, Walker MP. (2009) REM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memory. Cereb Cortex 19:1158-1166. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn155
![]() |
[4] |
Payne JD, Stickgold R, Swanberg K, Kensinger EA. (2008) Sleep preferentially enhances memory for emotional components of scenes. Psychol Sci 19: 781-788. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02157.x
![]() |
[5] | Payne JD, Chambers AM, Kensinger EA. (2012) Sleep promotes lasting changes in selective memory for emotional scenes. Front Integrative Neurosci 6(108): DOI:10. 3389/fnint. 2012. 00108. |
[6] |
Braun AR, Balkin TJ, Wesenten NJ, Carson RE, Varga M, Baldwin P, et al. (1997) Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle: An H215O PET study. Brain 120:1173-1197. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.7.1173
![]() |
[7] |
Stickgold R, Hobson JA, Fosse R, Fosse M. (2001). Sleep, learning, and dreams: Off-line memory reprocessing. Science 294: 1052-1057. doi: 10.1126/science.1063530
![]() |
[8] |
Plihal W, Born J. (1999) Memory consolidation in human sleep depends on inhibition of glucocorticoid release. NeuroReport 10: 2741-2747. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199909090-00009
![]() |
[9] | Gais S, Born J. (2004) Low acetylcholine during slow-wave sleep is critical for declarative memory consolidation. P Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 2140–2144. |
[10] |
Wagner U, Hallschmid M, Rasch B, Born J. (2006) Brief sleep after learning keeps emotional memories alive for years. Biol Psychiat 60: 788-790. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.061
![]() |
[11] |
Heller AS, Johnstone T, Shackman AJ, et al. (2009) Reduced capacity to sustain positive emotion in major depression reflects diminished maintenance of fronto-striatal brain activation. P Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 22445-22450. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910651106
![]() |
[12] |
Gruber J, Oveis C, Keltner D, Johnson SL. (2011) A discrete emotions approach to positive emotion disturbance in depression. Cognition Emotion 25: 40-52. doi: 10.1080/02699931003615984
![]() |
[13] |
Tsuno N, Besset A, Ritchie K. (2005) Sleep and depression. J Clin Psychiat 66: 1254-1269. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v66n1008
![]() |
[14] |
Lang A, Dhillon K, Dong Q. (1995) The effects of emotional arousal and valence on television viewers' cognitive capacity and memory. J Broadcast Electron 39: 313-327. doi: 10.1080/08838159509364309
![]() |
[15] |
Hamann SB, Ely TD, Grafton ST, Kilts CD. (1999) Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nat Neurosci 2: 289-293. doi: 10.1038/6404
![]() |
[16] |
Libkumen TM, Stabler CL, Otani H. (2004) Arousal, valence, and memory for detail. Memory 12:237-247. doi: 10.1080/09658210244000630
![]() |
[17] |
Hamann S. (2001) Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends Cogn Sci 5:394-400. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01707-1
![]() |
[18] |
Hamann SB, Ely TD, Hoffman JM, Kilts CD. (2002) Ecstasy and agony: Activation of human amygdala in positive and negative emotion. Psychol Sci 13: 135-141. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00425
![]() |
[19] |
Waring JD, Kensinger EA. (2009) Effects of emotional valence and arousal upon memory trade-offs with aging. Psychol Aging 24: 412-422. doi: 10.1037/a0015526
![]() |
[20] |
Waring JD, Kensinger EA. (2011) How emotion leads to selective memory: Neuroimaging evidence. Neuropsychologia 49: 1831-1842. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.007
![]() |
[21] |
Payne JD, Kensinger EA. (2011) Sleep leads to changes in the emotional memory trace: Evidence from fMRI. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 1285-1297. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21526
![]() |
[22] | Payne JD, Kensinger EA. (2010) Sleep's role in the consolidation of emotional episodic memories. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19: 290-295 |
[23] | Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson AL, Quan SF, eds. (2007) The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology, and Technical Specifications. 1 Eds. , Westchester, Illinois: American Academy of Sleep Medicine. |
[24] |
Yonelinas AP. (2002) The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. J Mem Lang 46: 441-517. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2002.2864
![]() |
[25] | Yonelinas AP, Jacoby LL. (1995) The relation between remembering and knowing as bases for recognition: Effects of size congruency. J Mem Lang 34: 622–643. |
1. | Sara Y. Kim, Sarah M. Kark, Ryan T. Daley, Sara E. Alger, Daniella Rebouças, Elizabeth A. Kensinger, Jessica D. Payne, Interactive effects of stress reactivity and rapid eye movement sleep theta activity on emotional memory formation, 2020, 30, 1050-9631, 829, 10.1002/hipo.23138 | |
2. | Sarah K. Schäfer, Benedikt E. Wirth, Marlene Staginnus, Nicolas Becker, Tanja Michael, M. Roxanne Sopp, Sleep's impact on emotional recognition memory: A meta-analysis of whole-night, nap, and REM sleep effects, 2020, 51, 10870792, 101280, 10.1016/j.smrv.2020.101280 | |
3. | Dan Denis, Sara Y. Kim, Sarah M. Kark, Ryan T. Daley, Elizabeth A. Kensinger, Jessica D. Payne, Slow oscillation‐spindle coupling is negatively associated with emotional memory formation following stress, 2021, 0953-816X, 10.1111/ejn.15132 | |
4. | Ateka A. Contractor, Danica C. Slavish, Nicole H. Weiss, Ahmad M. Alghraibeh, Ali A. Alafnan, Daniel J. Taylor, Moderating effects of sleep difficulties on relations between posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and positive memory count, 2021, 0021-9762, 10.1002/jclp.23142 | |
5. | Sophie Schwartz, Alice Clerget, Lampros Perogamvros, Enhancing imagery rehearsal therapy for nightmares with targeted memory reactivation, 2022, 32, 09609822, 4808, 10.1016/j.cub.2022.09.032 | |
6. | Dan Denis, Kristin E. G. Sanders, Elizabeth A. Kensinger, Jessica D. Payne, Sleep preferentially consolidates negative aspects of human memory: Well-powered evidence from two large online experiments, 2022, 119, 0027-8424, 10.1073/pnas.2202657119 | |
7. | Per Davidson, Peter Jönsson, Ingegerd Carlsson, Edward Pace-Schott, Does Sleep Selectively Strengthen Certain Memories Over Others Based on Emotion and Perceived Future Relevance?, 2021, Volume 13, 1179-1608, 1257, 10.2147/NSS.S286701 | |
8. | Esther Yuet Ying Lau, Mark Lawrence Wong, Yeuk Ching Lam, Kristy Nga Ting Lau, Ka Fai Chung, Benjamin Rusak, Sleep and Inhibitory Control Over Mood-Congruent Information in Emerging Adults With Depressive Disorder, 2021, 83, 1534-7796, 1004, 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000996 | |
9. | Xinran Niu, Mia F. Utayde, Kristin E. G. Sanders, Dan Denis, Elizabeth A. Kensinger, Jessica D. Payne, Age-related positivity effect in emotional memory consolidation from middle age to late adulthood, 2024, 18, 1662-5153, 10.3389/fnbeh.2024.1342589 |
Measure | Sleep | Wake | t-value | p-value |
All comparisons were non-significant. | ||||
Negative Affect | 13.58 | 13.39 | 0.17 | 0.86 |
Positive Affect | 24.61 | 22.71 | 1.05 | 0.30 |
STAI | 38.61 | 37.68 | 0.44 | 0.66 |
PSQI | 4.68 | 4.26 | 0.81 | 0.42 |
BDI | 4.59 | 3.81 | 0.84 | 0.40 |
Measure | AM | PM | t-value | p-value |
All comparisons were non-significant. | ||||
Negative Affect | 14.75 | 13.29 | 0.72 | 0.48 |
Positive Affect | 24.81 | 25.94 | -0.43 | 0.67 |
STAI | 41.37 | 37.41 | 1.31 | 0.20 |
PSQI | 4.63 | 4.88 | -0.28 | 0.78 |
BDI | 6.75 | 4.47 | 1.26 | 0.22 |
Sleep | Wake | t-value | |||
* = p < 0.05 | |||||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
Positive Objects | 0.69 | 0.21 | 0.58 | 0.21 | 2.09 * |
Neutral Objects | 0.37 | 0.18 | 0.29 | 0.21 | 1.77 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.45 | 0.21 | 0.36 | 0.17 | 1.82 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.47 | 0.17 | 0.39 | 0.19 | 1.59 |
Sleep | Wake | t-value | |||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
All comparisons were non-significant | |||||
Positive Objects | 0.58 | 0.31 | 0.61 | 0.25 | -0.34 |
Neutral Objects | 0.37 | 0.23 | 0.36 | 0.22 | 0.13 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.40 | 0.24 | 0.35 | 0.15 | 1.01 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.47 | 0.21 | 0.41 | 0.18 | 1.10 |
AM Control | PM Control | t-value | |||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
All comparisons were non-significant | |||||
Positive Objects | 0.83 | 0.17 | 0.85 | 0.15 | -0.38 |
Neutral Objects | 0.51 | 0.23 | 0.58 | 0.23 | -0.80 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.58 | 0.16 | 0.67 | 0.22 | -1.28 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.55 | 0.16 | 0.64 | 0.18 | -1.44 |
AM Control | PM Control | t-value | |||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
All comparisons were non-significant | |||||
Positive Objects | 0.59 | 0.36 | 0.45 | 0.38 | 1.02 |
Neutral Objects | 0.39 | 0.24 | 0.43 | 0.26 | -0.42 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.33 | 0.14 | 0.35 | 0.26 | -0.26 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.38 | 0.21 | 0.42 | 0.25 | -0.51 |
Measure | Sleep | Wake | t-value | p-value |
All comparisons were non-significant. | ||||
Negative Affect | 13.58 | 13.39 | 0.17 | 0.86 |
Positive Affect | 24.61 | 22.71 | 1.05 | 0.30 |
STAI | 38.61 | 37.68 | 0.44 | 0.66 |
PSQI | 4.68 | 4.26 | 0.81 | 0.42 |
BDI | 4.59 | 3.81 | 0.84 | 0.40 |
Measure | AM | PM | t-value | p-value |
All comparisons were non-significant. | ||||
Negative Affect | 14.75 | 13.29 | 0.72 | 0.48 |
Positive Affect | 24.81 | 25.94 | -0.43 | 0.67 |
STAI | 41.37 | 37.41 | 1.31 | 0.20 |
PSQI | 4.63 | 4.88 | -0.28 | 0.78 |
BDI | 6.75 | 4.47 | 1.26 | 0.22 |
Sleep | Wake | t-value | |||
* = p < 0.05 | |||||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
Positive Objects | 0.69 | 0.21 | 0.58 | 0.21 | 2.09 * |
Neutral Objects | 0.37 | 0.18 | 0.29 | 0.21 | 1.77 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.45 | 0.21 | 0.36 | 0.17 | 1.82 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.47 | 0.17 | 0.39 | 0.19 | 1.59 |
Sleep | Wake | t-value | |||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
All comparisons were non-significant | |||||
Positive Objects | 0.58 | 0.31 | 0.61 | 0.25 | -0.34 |
Neutral Objects | 0.37 | 0.23 | 0.36 | 0.22 | 0.13 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.40 | 0.24 | 0.35 | 0.15 | 1.01 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.47 | 0.21 | 0.41 | 0.18 | 1.10 |
AM Control | PM Control | t-value | |||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
All comparisons were non-significant | |||||
Positive Objects | 0.83 | 0.17 | 0.85 | 0.15 | -0.38 |
Neutral Objects | 0.51 | 0.23 | 0.58 | 0.23 | -0.80 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.58 | 0.16 | 0.67 | 0.22 | -1.28 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.55 | 0.16 | 0.64 | 0.18 | -1.44 |
AM Control | PM Control | t-value | |||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
All comparisons were non-significant | |||||
Positive Objects | 0.59 | 0.36 | 0.45 | 0.38 | 1.02 |
Neutral Objects | 0.39 | 0.24 | 0.43 | 0.26 | -0.42 |
Positive Backgrounds | 0.33 | 0.14 | 0.35 | 0.26 | -0.26 |
Neutral Backgrounds | 0.38 | 0.21 | 0.42 | 0.25 | -0.51 |