Citation: Chiyoko Kobayashi Frank. Reviving pragmatic theory of theory of mind[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(2): 116-131. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.2.116
[1] | Dennet DC (1980) The milk of human intentionality (commentary on Searle). Behav Brain Sci 3: 428–430. |
[2] | Frith U, Frith CD (2003) Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philos Trans R Soc London 358: 459–473. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218 |
[3] | Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (1985) Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"? Cognition 21: 37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 |
[4] | Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (1986) Mechanical, behavioural and Intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. Br J Dev Psychol 4: 113–125. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1986.tb01003.x |
[5] | Brüne M (2005) "Theory of mind" in schizophrenia: A review of the literature. Schizophrenia Bull 31: 21–42. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi002 |
[6] | Frith CD (1992) The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Hove, England: Psychology Press. |
[7] | Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, et al. (2007) Theory of mind in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 191: 5–13. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.035899 |
[8] | Washburn D, Wilson G, Rose M, et al. (2016) Theory of mind in social anxiety disorder, depression, and comorbid conditions. J Anxiety Disord 37: 71–77. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.11.004 |
[9] | Vaskinn A, Antonsen BT, Fretland RA, et al. (2015) Theory of mind in women with borderline personality disorder or schizophrenia: Differences in overall ability and error patterns. Front Psychol 6: 1239. |
[10] | Ferstl EC, Neumann J, Bogler C, et al. (2008) The extended language network: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Hum Brain Mapp 29: 581–593. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20422 |
[11] | Grice HP (1969) Utterer's Meaning and Intentions. Philos Rev 78: 147–177. doi: 10.2307/2184179 |
[12] | Grice HP (1975) Logic and conversation, In: Cole P, Morgan JL, eds., Speech Acts, New York, NY: Academic Press, 41–58. |
[13] | Grice HP (1989) Studies in the Way of Words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. |
[14] | Ruiz-Gurillo L, Alvarado-Ortega MB (2013) The pragmatics of irony and humor, In: Ruiz-Gurillo L, Alvarado-Ortega MB, eds., Irony and humor: From pragmatics to discourse, Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins, 1–14. |
[15] | Van Ackeren MJ, Casasanto D, Bekkering H, et al. (2012) Pragmatics in action: Indirect requests engage theory of mind areas and the cortical motor network. Cognit Neurosci J 24: 2237–2247. |
[16] | Austin JA (1962) How to do things with words. Analysis 23: 58–64. |
[17] | Searle JR (1969) Seech acts. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. |
[18] | Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindreading: An essay on autism and theory of mind. London, England: MIT Press. |
[19] | Fodor JA (1975) The language of thought. New York, NY: Crowell. 22: 69–71. |
[20] | Leslie AM, Friedman O, German TP (2004) Core mechanism in "theory of mind". Trends Cognit Sci 8: 528–533. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.001 |
[21] | Astington JW, Jenkins JM (1999) A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory of mind development. Dev Psychol 35: 1311–1320. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311 |
[22] | De Villiers JG, De Villiers PA (2000) Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs, In: Mitchell P, Riggs KJ, eds., Children's reasoning and the mind, East Sussex, England: Psychology Press, 191–228. |
[23] | Milligan K, Astington JW, Dack LA (2007) Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Dev 78: 622–648. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x |
[24] | Mar RA (2011) The neural basis of social cognition and story comprehension. Ann Rev Psychol 62: 103–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145406 |
[25] | Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006) Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 268–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn1884 |
[26] | Krall SC, Rottschy C, Overwelland E, et al. (2015) The role of the right temporoprietal junction in attention and social interaction as revealed by ALE meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct 220: 587–604. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0803-z |
[27] | Kobayashi C, Glover GH, Temple E (2006) Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of "Theory of Mind": An fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals. Brain Lang 98: 210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.013 |
[28] | Kobayashi C, Glover GH, Temple E (2007) Children's and adults' neural bases of verbal and nonverbal "Theory of Mind". Neuropsychologia 45: 1522–1532. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.017 |
[29] | Frank CK, Baron-Cohen S, Ganzel BL (2015) Sex differences in the neural basis of false-belief and pragmatic language comprehension. NeuroImage 105: 300–311. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.041 |
[30] | Saxe R, Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind: The role of the right tempro-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 43: 1391–1399. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.02.013 |
[31] | Frith U (2003) Autism: Explaining the enigma. Malden, MA: Blackwell. |
[32] | Happé FG (1993) Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition 48: 101–119. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90026-R |
[33] | Tager-Flusberg H (2000) Language and Understanding Minds: Connections in autism, In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ, eds., Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neuroscience, 2nd Edition., Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, 124–149. |
[34] | Corcoran R, Frith CD (2003) Autobiographical memory and theory of mind: Evidence of a relationship in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 33: 897–905. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703007529 |
[35] | Gavilan IJM, Garcia-Albea RJE (2013) Theory of mind and language comprehension in schizophrenia. Psiothema 25: 440–445. |
[36] | Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2009) Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition 112: 337–342. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006 |
[37] | Buttleman D, Over H, Carpenter M, et al. (2014) Eighteen-month olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected contents task. J Exp Child Psychol 119: 120–126. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.002 |
[38] | He Z, Bolz M, Baillargeon R (2012) 2.5-year-olds succeed at a verbal anticipatory-looking false-belief task. Br J Dev Psychol 30: 14–29. |
[39] | Onishi KH, Baillargeon R (2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false belief? Science 308: 255–258. doi: 10.1126/science.1107621 |
[40] | Wellman HM, Cross D, Watson J (2001) Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false-belief. Child Dev 72: 655–684. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00304 |
[41] | Baillargeon R, Scott RM, He Z (2010) False-belief understanding in infants. Trends Cognit Sci 14: 110–118. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006 |
[42] | Low J, Perner J (2012) Implicit and explicit theory of mind: State of the art. Br J Dev Psychol 30: 1–13. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02074.x |
[43] | Sperber D, Wilson D (2002) Pragmatics, modularity, and mindreading. Mind Lang 17: 3–23. doi: 10.1111/1468-0017.00186 |
[44] | Malle BF (2002) The relation between language and theory of mind in development and evolution, In: Givón T, Malle BF, eds., The evolution of language out of pre-language, Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Benjamins, 265–284. |
[45] | Westra E, Carruthers P (2017) Pragmatic development explains the theory-of-mind scale. Cognition 158: 165–176. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.021 |
[46] | Leslie AM (1992) Autism and the "theory of mind" module. Cur Dir Psych Sci 1: 18–21. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10767818 |
[47] | Roth D, Leslie AM (1998) Solving belief problems: Towards a task analysis. Cognition 66: 1–31. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00005-5 |
[48] | Gopnik A, Wellman HM (1992) Why the child's theory of mind really is a theory. Mind Lang 7: 145–171. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00202.x |
[49] | Goldman AI (1989) Interpretation psychologized. Mind Lang 4: 161–185 doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.1989.tb00249.x |
[50] | Harris PL (1992) From simulation to folk psychology: The case for development. Mind Lang 7: 120–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00201.x |
[51] | Cummings L (2013) Clinical pragmatics and theory of mind. Springer Int Publishing 2: 23–56. doi: 10.1186/2193-1801-2-23 |
[52] | Frank CK (2010) Linguistic effects on the neural basis of theory of mind. Open Neuroimaging J 4: 37–45. doi: 10.2174/1874440001004020037 |
[53] | Frank CK (2012) Cultural and linguistic influence on developmental neural basis of theory of mind: Whorfian hypothesis revisited. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers. |
[54] | Saxe R (2005) Against simulation: The argument from error. Trends Cogit Sci 9: 174–179. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.012 |
[55] | Scholl BJ, Leslie AM (1999) Modularity, development and "Theory of Mind". Mind Lang 14: 131–153. doi: 10.1111/1468-0017.00106 |
[56] | Zufferey S (2010) Lexical pragmatics and theory of mind. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. |
[57] | Surian L, Caldi S, Sperber D (2007) Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychol Sci 18: 580–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x |
[58] | Helming KA, Strickland B, Jacob P (2016) Solving the puzzle about early belief ascription. Mind Lang 31: 438–469. doi: 10.1111/mila.12114 |
[59] | Airenti G (2017) Pragmatic Development, In: Cummings L, ed., Research in Pragmatics, Cham, Switzerland: Springer, 3–28. |
[60] | Malloch SN, Sharp DB, Campbell AM, et al. (1997) Measuring the human voice: Analyzing pitch, timing, loudness and voice quality in mother/infant communication. Proc Inst Acoustics 19: 495–500. |
[61] | Trevarthen C, Kokkinaki T, Fiamenghi GA (1999) What infants' imitations communicate: With mothers with fathers and with peers, In: Nadel J, Butterworth G, eds., Imitation in infancy, Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 127–185. |
[62] | Butterworth GE, Cochran E (1980) Toward a mechanism of joint visual attention in human infancy. Int J Behav Dev 3: 253–272. |
[63] | Stern HH (1975) What can we learn from the good language learner? Can Mod Lang Rev 34: 304–318. |
[64] | Bates E, Camaion L, Volterra V (1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21: 205–226. |
[65] | Bruner JS (1975) The ontogenesis of speech acts. J Child Lang 2: 1–19. |
[66] | Liszkowsku U, Brown P, Callaghan T, et al. (2012) A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognit Sci 36: 698–713. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01228.x |
[67] | Friederici AD, Rüschemeyer SA, Hahne A, et al. (2003) The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: Localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cereb Cortex 13: 117–177. |
[68] | Martin I, McDonalds S (2005) Exploring the cause of pragmatic deficits following traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology 19: 712–730. doi: 10.1080/02687030500172203 |
[69] | Surian L, Siegal M (2001) Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in the right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Lang 78: 224–232. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2465 |
[70] | Weed E, Mcgregor W, Nielsen JF, et al. (2010) Theory of Mind in adults with right hemisphere data: What's the story? Brain Lang 113: 65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.01.009 |
[71] | Weylman ST, Brownell HH, Roman M, et al. (1989) Appreciation of indirect requests by left- and right-brain damaged patients: The effect of verbal context and conventionality of wording. Brain Lang 36: 580–591. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(89)90087-4 |
[72] | Brundage S (1996) Comparison of proverb interpretations provided by right-hemisphere damaged adults and adults with probable dementia of the Alzheimer type. Clin Aphasiol 24: 215–231. |
[73] | Cutica I, Bucciarelli M, Bara BG (2006) Neuropragmatics: Extralinguistic pragmatic ability is better preserved in left-hemisphere-damaged patients than in right-hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Lang 98: 12–25. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.01.001 |
[74] | Cheang H, Pell M (2006) A study of humour and communicative intention following right hemisphere stroke. Clin Linguist Phonetics 20: 447–462. doi: 10.1080/02699200500135684 |
[75] | Winner E, Brownell H, Happe F, et al. (1998) Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain damaged patients. Brain Lang 62: 89–106. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1889 |
[76] | Mcdonald S (2000) Exploring the cognitive basis of right-hemisphere pragmatic language disorders. Brain Lang 75: 82–107. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2342 |
[77] | Kucharskapietura K, Phillips ML, Germand W, et al. (2003) Perceptions of emotion from faces and voices following unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia 41: 1082–1090. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00294-4 |
[78] | Parola A, Gabbatore I, Bosco FM, et al. (2016) Assessment of pragmatic impairment in right hemisphere damage. J Neurolinguistics 39: 10–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.12.003 |
[79] | Shamaytsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, et al. (2005) Impaired "affective theory of mind" is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cognit Behav Neurol 18: 55–62. |
[80] | Sidtis DVL, Yang S (2016) Formulaic language performance in left- and right-hemisphere damaged patients: Structured testing. Aphasiology 31: 82–99. |
[81] | Heine B, Kuteva T, Kaltenöck G (2014) Discourse, grammar, the dual process model, and brain lateralization: Some correlations. Lang Cognit 6: 146–180. doi: 10.1017/langcog.2013.3 |
[82] | Heine B, Kuteva T, Kaltenöck G, et al. (2015) On some correlations between grammar and brain lateralization, In: Oxford Handbooks Online, Oxford, England: OUP. |
[83] | Brownell H, Griffin R, Winner E, et al. (2000) Cerebral lateralization and theory of mind, In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen D, eds., Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience, 2, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 306–333. |
[84] | Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, et al. (2006) Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality 11: 195–225. doi: 10.1080/13576500500450552 |
[85] | Siegal M, Carrington J, Radel M (1996) Theory of mind understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain Lang 53: 40–50. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0035 |
[86] | Weed E (2011) What's left to learn about right hemisphere damage and pragmatic impairment? Aphasiology 25: 872–889. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2010.545423 |
[87] | Champagne-Lavau M, Joanette Y (2009) Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: Different patterns of deficits. J Neurolinguistics 22: 413–426. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.02.002 |
[88] | Frith CD, Frith U (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50: 531–534. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001 |
[89] | Kobayashi C, Glover GH, Temple E (2008) Switching language switches mind: Linguistic effects on developmental neural bases of "Theory of Mind". Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 3: 62–70. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm039 |
[90] | Ferstl EC, Cramon DYV (2002) What does the frontomedian cortex contribute to language processing: Coherence or theory of mind? Neuroimage 17: 1599–1612. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1247 |
[91] | Bašnáková J, Weber K, Petersson KM, et al. (2014) Beyond the language given: The neural correlates of inferring speaker meaning. Cereb Cortex 24: 2572–2578. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht112 |
[92] | Spotorno N, Koun E, Prado J, et al. (2012) Neural evidence that utterance-processing entails mentalizing: The case of irony. NeuroImage 63: 25–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.046 |
[93] | Castelli F, Frith C, Happé F, et al. (2002) Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states of animated shapes. Brain 125: 1839–1849. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf189 |
[94] | Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of "theory of mind". Trends Cognit Sci 7: 77–83. |
[95] | Barrett HC, Kurzban R (2006) Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychol Rev 113: 628–647. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.3.628 |
[96] | Fodor JA (1983) The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
[97] | Scholl BJ, Leslie AM (2001) Minds, modules, and meta-analysis. Child Dev 72: 696–701. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00308 |
[98] | Carlson SM, Moses LJ, Claxton LJ (2004) Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. J Exp Child Psychol 87: 299–319. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.002 |
[99] | Perner J (1991) Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
[100] | Karmiloffsmith A (2015) An alternative to domain-general or domain-specific frameworks for theorizing about human evolution and ontogenesis. AIMS Neurosci 2: 91–104. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.2.91 |
[101] | Trosborg A (2010) Pragmatics across language and culture. Berlin, Germany: Walter de Gruyter. |
[102] | Zhu J, Bao Y (2010) The pragmatic comparison of Chinese and Western "Politeness" in cross-cultural communication. J Lang Teach Res 1: 848–851. |
[103] | Wierzbicka A (2003) Cross-cultural pragmatics: The semantics of human interaction. Berlin, Germany: Walter de Gruyter. |
[104] | Wierzbicka A (2010) Cultural scripts and intercultural communication, In: Trosborg A, ed., Pragmatics across language and culture, Berlin, Germany: Walter de Gruyter, 43–78. |
[105] | Liu D, Wellman HM, Tardif T, et al. (2008) Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Dev Psychol 44: 523–531. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.523 |
[106] | Kobayashi FC, Temple E (2009) Cultural effects on the neural basis of theory of mind. Prog Brain Res 178: 213–223. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17815-9 |
[107] | Rowley DA, Rogish M, Alexander T, et al. (2017) Cognitive correlates of pragmatic language comprehension in adult traumatic brain injury: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Inj 31: 1564–1574. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2017.1341645 |
[108] | Landa R (2007) Early communication development and intervention for children with autism. Dev Disabil Res Rev 13: 16–25. |
[109] | Seung HK, Siddiqi S, Elder JH (2006) Intervention outcomes of a bilingual child with autism. J Med Speech Lang Pathol 14: 53–63. |
[110] | Thunberg G, Johansson M, Wikholm J (2015) Meeting the communicative rights of people with autism-using pictorial supports during assessment, intervention and hospital care, Chapter 15, Open Access, In: Fizgerald M, ed., Autism spectrum disorder-Recent advances, London, England: In Tech. |
[111] | Rhodes M, Wellman H (2013) Constructing a new theory from old ideas and new evidence. Cognit Sci 37: 592–604. |
[112] | Ludgren K, Brownell H (2015) Selective training of theory of mind in traumatic brain injury: A series of single subject training studies. Open Behav Sci J 9: 1–11. doi: 10.2174/1874230001509010001 |
[113] | Wellman H, Liu D (2004) Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Dev 75: 523–541. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x |
[114] | Brown JW (1977) Mind, brain and consciousness. New York, NY: Academic Press. |
[115] | Brown JW (2015) Microgenetic theory and process thought. New York, NY: Academic Press. |
[116] | Siegler R, Stern E (1998) Conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries: A micrognetic analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen 127: 377–397. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.127.4.377 |
[117] | Howlin P (2006) Augmentative and alternative communication systems for children with autism, In: Charman T, Stone W, eds., Social and communication development in autism spectrum disorders, New York, NY: Guilford Press, 236–266. |
[118] | Levinson SC (1984) Pragmatics (Cambridge textbooks in linguistics). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. |
[119] | Blakemore SJ, Decety J (2001) From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 561–567. doi: 10.1038/35080587 |
[120] | Iacoboni M, Dapretto M (2006) The mirror neuron system and the consequence of its dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 942–951. doi: 10.1038/nrn2024 |
[121] | Haugh M (2008) The place of intention in the interactional achievement of implicature, In: Kecskes I, Mey J, eds., Intention, common ground and the egocentric speaker-hearer, Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter, 45–86. |
[122] | Charman T, Baroncohen S, Swetternham J, et al. (2000) Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognit Dev 15: 481–498. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00037-5 |
[123] | Rapp AM, Felsenheimer AK, Langohr K, et al. (2018) The comprehension of familiar and novel metaphoric meanings in schizophrenia: A pilot study. Front Psychol 8: 2251. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02251 |
[124] | Varga E, Simon M, Tényi T, et al. (2013) Irony comprehension and context processing in schizophrenia in remission-a functional MRI study. Brain Lang 126: 231–242. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.017 |
[125] | Mo S, Su Y, Chan RC, et al. (2008) Comprehension of metaphor and irony in schizophrenia during remission: The role of theory of mind and IQ. Psychiatry Res 157: 21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.04.002 |
[126] | Wang AT, Lee SS, Sigman M, et al. (2006) Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain 129: 932–943. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl032 |
[127] | Apperly IA, Butterfill SA (2009) Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief like states? Psychol Rev 116: 953–970. doi: 10.1037/a0016923 |
[128] | Bloom P, German TP (2000) Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition 77: B25–B31. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00096-2 |
[129] | Sabbagh MA, Xu F, Carison SM, et al. (2006) The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychol Sci 17: 74–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.x |